Women’s Bragging Rights: Overcoming Modesty Norms to Facilitate Women’s Self- Promotion Author: Jessi Smith & Meghan Huntoon  This is the peer reviewed version of the folowing article: Smith, Jessi L., and Meghan Huntoon. "Women’s Bragging Rights Overcoming Modesty Norms to Facilitate Women’s Self-Promotion." Psychology of Women Quarterly 38 no. 4 (2013): 447-459, which has been published in final form at DOI: 10.1177/0361684313515840. This article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance With Wiley Terms and Conditions for self-archiving. Psychology of Women Quarterly Smith, Jessi L., and Meghan Huntoon. "Women’s Bragging Rights Overcoming Modesty Norms to Facilitate Women’s Self-Promotion." Psychology of Women Quarterly 38 no. 4 (2013): 447-459. DOI: 10.1177/0361684313515840. Made available through Montana State University’s ScholarWorks  scholarworks.montana.edu  Women’s Bragging Rights: Overcoming Modesty Norms to Facilitate Women’s Self- Promotion Jesi L. Smith and Meghan Huntoon: Department of Psychology, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT Abstract Within American gender norms is the expectation that women should be modest. We argue that violating this ‘modesty norm’ by boasting about one’s accomplishments causes women to experience uncomfortable situational arousal that leads to lower motivation for and performance on a self-promotion task. We hypothesized that such negative efects could be ofset when an external source for their situational arousal was made available. To test hypotheses, 78 women students from a U.S. Northwestern university wrote a scholarship application essay to promote the merits of either the self (modesty norm vio-lated) or another person as a leter of reference (modesty norm not violated). Half were randomly assigned to hear informa-tion about a (fake) subliminal noise generator in the room that might cause ‘discomfort’ (misatribution available) and half were told nothing about the generator (normal condition: misatribution not available). Participants rated the task and 44 new naive participants judged how much scholarship money to award each essay. Results confirmed predictions: under normal conditions, violating the modesty norm led to decreased motivation and performance. However, those who violated the mod-esty norm with a misatribution source reported increased interest, adopted fewer performance-avoidance goals, perceived their own work to be of higher quality, and produced higher quality work. Results suggest that when a situation helps women to escape the discomfort of defying the modesty norm, self-promotion motivation and performance improve. Further impli-cations for enhancing women’s academic and workplace experiences are discussed. Keywords social norms, motivation, atribution, performance, modesty, impression management Several years ago, we were puting together a magazine fea-turing the achievements of women faculty on campus. We put out a cal for women to share their successes and accomplish-ments in teaching or research or family life. We received no replies. We did, however, receive many replies from people teling us about other women we should feature and the good work that these other women on campus were doing. Exam-ples such as ours ilustrate that many women are fine with referencing the good works of others but are reticent to pro-mote themselves. Such possible discomfort likely stems, at least in part, from a lifelong process of learning gender norms (Blakemore, 2003), whereby girls are typicaly told to ‘act like a lady’ and be polite and quiet, take up litle space, and be modest (Alen, Gervais, & Smith, 2013; Blakemore, 2003). Girls are thus socialized to be modest (Bronstein, 2006; Leaper, Breed, Hofman, & Perlman, 2002), and research shows this modesty norm influences women’s adult professional careers (Budworth & Mann, 2010; Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999). Professional careers and aca-demic purists in the United States often require that people self-promote in order to achieve success. Therefore, conform-ing to modesty norms may prevent women from advancing or succeeding (e.g., negotiating a high starting salary; Smal, self-promote in order to achieve success. Therefore, conform-ing to modesty norms may prevent women from advancing or succeeding (e.g., negotiating a high starting salary; Smal,Gelfand, Babcock, & Getman, 2007; Wade, 2001). Thus, it is critical to understand ways to enhance women’s self-promotion without incuring norm-breaking backlash. The aim of the curent study was to empiricaly test whether women’s self-promotion performance and motivation could be protected against such norm-breaking discomfort using a misatribution intervention. We argue that breaking the gender modesty norm causes women to experience discomfort (manifested as situational arousal, anxiety, or fear). Certainly, one way to escape such discomfort is to downplay or opt out of self- promoting al together, thereby conforming to the modesty norm. Indeed, women often want to publicly appear modest (Gould & Slone, 1982). To be modest means having a moderate opinion of one’s self and lacking pretentiousness, as wel as denying responsibility for successes and accepting responsibility for failures (Gould & Slone, 1982; Moss-Racusin, Phelan, & Rudman, 2010). For example, women lower their claims of success in a public (vs. private) seting, whereas men’s claims of success are not influenced by the seting (Gould & Slone, 1982). This modesty norm is embedded within the communal orientation expectations for women. Indeed, women are socialized to highly value communal goals, such as the goal to help others and connect with others (Diekman, Brown, Johnston, & Clark, 2010). Women are therefore expected to be selfless, which is one of the components of modesty (Wade, 2001). Because women are expected to be selfless, they are also expected to advocate for others rather than for themselves. The implication then is that the modesty norm pressures women to advocate for rewards for other people, but not for themselves. Such cultural norms may be one reason why gender disparities persist in hiring practices, promotions, awards,raises, and other important workplace outcomes (Amanatulah & Moris, 2010; Rudman, 1998). Norm-Consistent and Inconsistent Behavior What happens when someone adheres to (vs. violates) a given gender norm? Adhering to gender norms can be beneficial. Women who behave in a norm-consistent manner are liked more than women who behave in a norm-inconsistent manner (Crawford, 1988). For example, a woman who displays sad- ness (a gender-appropriate emotion for women; Condry & Condry, 1976) in an interview seting is viewed more posi- tively than the same woman who displays anger—with the opposite being true for men (Brescol & Uhlmann, 2008). This positive perception of norm consistency translates to other important outcomes. For example, women who adhere to gender norms by not expressing anger are accorded higher status and higher wages, and they are seen as more competent than women who do express anger (Brescol & Uhlmann, 2008; Rudman & Glick, 2001). Whereas acting in a norm-consistent manner is often beneficial (Wade, 2001), violating a given norm is nega- tively regarded. People who behave in a manner inconsistent with gender norms often sufer interpersonal consequences (Brescol, 2011; Brescol & Uhlmann, 2008; Moss-Racusin et al., 2010; Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999). For example, even in circumstances where agentic qualities are necessary (e.g., management or CEO positions), women are more personaly derogated and disliked by others when they display agentic (masculine) characteristics (Brescol, 2011). This ‘backlash’ can ultimately result in agentic women experiencing hiring, promotion, and wage discrimination (Rudman & Glick, 1999). Women who boast about their own accomplishments violate the gender norm of modesty (Rudman, 1998). Such ‘self-promoting’ is defined as divulging one’s own accom- plishments and credentials. In an interview seting, where such self-promotion might be viewed as essential to success, women might find themselves in a double bind: If they self-promote, they are violating gender norms and might experience back- lash; if they do not self-promote, they might not convey their qualifications (Bowles & Babcock, 2012). Thus, even though the seting requires self-promotion, women’s violation of the gender norm may lead to negative consequences. Furthermore, women may be more likely to ‘police’ other women’s gender norm violation. As a demonstration of this patern, Rudman (1998) had women and men engage in an interview task with two other people. One of the appli- cants was a self-promoting woman and the second was either a self-promoting man or a self-efacing man. Participants were told they would be choosing one of the two applicants as their partner in a competitive game with the potential to win a cash prize. Results showed that only women found the self-promoting woman less competent, less socialy atrac- tive, and less hirable than the self-promoting man. What is more, women disproportionately chose the self-promoting man as their partner over the self-promoting woman; how- ever, men chose equaly between men and women. Impor- tantly, the results showed that when a self-promoting woman was up against a strong male applicant, she was less likely to get the job, especialy if women were making the hir- ing decision (Rudman, 1998). Not only do onlookers disparage women who violate gen- der norms, but women themselves often go to great lengths to avoid the potential backlash associated with norm violations, even when it goes against their self-interest (Brescol, 2011). For instance, women who are modest and communal should not be out for self-gain, and research shows that women do not negotiate as efectively as men—not because they are inherently unskiled at negotiations, but because women downplay their negotiation skils to avoid the backlash asso- ciated with gender norm violation (Amanatulah & Moris, 2010). In their research, women who self-promoted by nego- tiating on behalf of themselves (thus violating the modesty gender norm) conceded much more than those who were negotiating on behalf of others (which is consistent with a communal interdependent gender norm). Results suggest that women are not unable to negotiate, but instead women may not negotiate on their own behalf in order to avoid the conse- quences associated with norm violation. When women are promoting the accomplishments of a peer, this other-orientation does not violate the modesty gen- der norm. As evidence, Moss-Racusin and Rudman (2010) had participants either promote themselves (modesty norm violation) or promote a peer (modesty norm consistent). When women promoted the accomplishments of another per- son, they showed increased promotion success; in contrast, when asked to promote themselves, women were less suc- cessful and felt il at ease. Thus, similar to the patern we saw with negotiation research, it does not appear that women are incapable of promoting accomplishments; rather, women seem to hold back when it comes to specificaly promoting their ownaccomplishments. Additionaly, men in Moss-Racusin and Rudman’s study reported higher levels of success after self-promoting than women, suggesting that the bariers to self-promotion that are present for women do not extend to men. Women’s motivation for self-promoting seems to be bounded by specific contextual features, including situa- tional norms that suggest acceptance of the behavior versus norms that trigger backlash fears (Rudman, Moss-Racusin, Glick, & Phelan, 2012). Indeed, people are typicaly unin- terested in and do not value activities that are perceived to violate gender norms (Alen & Smith, 2011; Boiche, Plaza, Chalabaev, Guilet, & Sarrazin,2014; Diekman et al., 2010; Horgan & Smith, 2006). For example, heterosexual men who participated in a medical task setup as norm- inconsistent (i.e., a task typical of nurses) reported lower interest in the task compared to men who engaged in the same taskthat was presented as norm-consistent (i.e., a task typical of doctors; Alen & Smith, 2011). Certainly, stereo- types and social norms contribute to a person’s reduced motivation, and such reduced motivation in turn reinforces those very social norms—together creating a reciprocal feedback loop (Eagly, Wood, & Diekman, 2000; Rudman et al., 2012; Smith, 2004; Thoman, Smith, Brown, Chase, & Lee, 2013). One way that stereotypes and norms harm a person’s performance and motivation is through the adoption of performance-avoidance goals (Brodish & Devine, 2009; Smith, 2004; Smith, Sansone, & White, 2007). Contempo- rary achievement goal scholarship (Eliot & McGregor, 2001; Finney, Pieper, & Barron, 2004; Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, & Eliot, 2000) shows perfor- mance-avoidancegoals orient the person toward avoiding performance failure relative to others; alternatively, per- formance-approachgoals orient a person toward perform- ing wel relative to others. These goals are differentialy activated by salient contextual cues (Urdan, 1999). Thus, one question we ask in the present article is whether enga- ging in self-promotion undergender norm-consistent ver- sus inconsistent conditions influences women’s adoption of performance goals. Moreover, women’s lower self- promotion performance and motivation could potentialy correlate with performance-avoidance goal adoption given that these avoidance goals are associated with a host of maladaptive outcomes, including underperformance and decreased interest (Harackiewicz et al., 2000). If the self-promotion context is made more positive, women may adopt fewer performance-avoidance goals and instead adopt more performance-approach goals. Performance- approach goals are characterized by a mixture of both posi- tively and negatively valenced antecedents, including a fear of failure, high competence valuation, and competitiveness (Eliot & McGregor, 2001). Educational studies show a consis- tent link between performance-approach goals and increased academic performance, and sometimes, increased academic interest (Harackiewicz et al., 2000). A Situational Arousal Account of Modesty Norm Violation One potential way to make self-promoting more positive for womenisbyreducingsituationalarousal.Peopleoften experience situational arousal when they act inconsistently (Zanna & Cooper, 1974). Indeed, people are motivated to behave in a way to maximize consistency and to minimize inconsistency in cognitions, feelings, and actions (Shaw & Skolnick, 1973). In this context, consistency implies that the cognitions, feelings, and actions of an individual ‘match.’ For example, if a woman knows that norms dictate that women should not brag about their accomplishments yet she finds herself in a situation where she must self-promote, this ‘mismatched’ experience can trigger situational arousal. Such arousal is often experienced as discomfort, anxiety, fear, or nervousness, as wel as physiological symptoms such as increased perspiration or heart rate (Zanna & Cooper, 1974). Situational arousal can be misatributed, however, reducing the influence of the discomfort. Zanna and Cooper (1974) found that when provided with an alter- nate external source of arousal(i.e., a pil that ostensibly caused arousal), participants induced to experience high cognitive dissonance were less likely to feel they needed to act consistently and were les likely to change their opi- nions to match their behavior. Because participants were able to misatribute the arousal associated with cognitive dissonance to the pil, they did not have to adjust their opi- nions in order to aleviate the arousal caused (in this case) by cognitive dissonance. Their results suggest that when situa- tional arousal is misatributed to an alternate external source, the need to act consistently is aleviated (Cooper, 1998; see also Cooper, Zanna, & Taves, 1978; Zanna & Cooper, 1974). Misatributing situational arousal is a potential way, then, for someone to overcome the need to act consistently. Less is known about whether such misatributions of arousal can work to overcome deeply embedded anxiety and fear result- ing from cultural gender normsand expectations. Two stud- ies on stereotype threat and misatribution do offer promising evidence; in both studies, results showed it was possible for women to overcome the anxiety associated with taking a stereotype-threatening test (in which the norm is for women to fail the exam) and improve their performance when an alternative source for arousal was provided (Ben-Zeev, Fein, & Inzlicht, 2005; Johns, Schmader, & Martens, 2005). Typicaly, when women are in a stereotype- relevant situation (such as when taking a math test) even subtle cues such as marking their gender on the test, or being outnumbered by men, can trigger stereotype threat and lead to lower performance (Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000; Smith et al., 2007; Smith & White, 2002). Yet, when women were given an external source to misatribute their arousal (they were taught that the stereotype itself is the source of anxiety or shown a subliminal noise generator that supposedly emited an arousal-causing noise), women performed beter on the math test compared to women who were not given an external source (Ben-Zeev et al., 2005; Johns et al., 2005). Thus, it appears that misatributing arousal might serve as one pathway to reduce the discomfort associated with violating the gender modesty norm and thus free women to engage in high-level self-promotion. Study Overview Given that the desire to behave consistently with the mod- esty norm acts as a barrier to self-promotion in women (Moss-Racusin et al., 2010; Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 1999), our project aimed to test whether women who violate the modesty norm but are given an opportunity to misatribute any arousal would show an improvement in their self-promotion. We compared these results to a condi- tion in which women completed a similar task but did not break the modesty norm—namely by promoting the accomplishments of someone else. We assessed both moti- vation (interest, performance-approach goal adoption, and performance-avoidance goal adoption) and promotion per- formance (actual and self-perceived). We also included two potential control variables: length of the promotion essay in terms of word count and particpants’ ratings about the perceived value of the promotion task. This reasoning led us to propose two hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that we would replicate the effect of the modesty norm within the normal (no misatribution avail- able condition) such that self-promoting women who vio- lated the modesty norm would be less motivated and wouldperformworsethanwomenwhopromotedonbehalf of another (Hypothesis 1). Second, we hypothesized that the negative impact of violating the modesty norm would be atenuated by misatribution availability whereby the moti- vation and performance gap between self-promoting and other-promoting women would close when self-promoting women were provided with a misatribution source for any situational arousal (Hypothesis 2). Breaking down our second hypothesis, we predicted that when provided with our misatribution-available experimen- tal condition, women who violated the modesty norm would show equal or improved motivation and performance com- pared to women who did not violate the modesty norm, thus closing the modesty norm gap found under normal conditions (Hypothesis 2a). Furthermore, we predicted that misatribution availability would enhance motivation and performance for women who violated the modesty norm com- pared to women who violated the modesty norm but were not provided with an alternate external misatribution source (Hypothesis 2b). Finaly, we anticipated that the misatribution source would have no efect on women promoting on behalf of another person (Prediction 2c). In sum, we expected that women in the norm violation condition without a misatribution source would difer from the remaining three cels of our 2 (modesty norm violated vs. not violated) 2 (misatribution available vs. not available) design. Method Participants and Study Design Participants were 78 women (Mage¼19.78 years, standard deviation¼3.24, range¼18–42; 90.6%White; 2.1%African American; 1% Asian; 1% Latino; 2.1% Native American; 3.2%unreported) introductory psychology students who parti- cipated for partial course credit. Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (modesty norm vio- lated vs. not violated) 2 (misatribution available vs. not available) between-participants design. There were 39 women in each of the misatribution conditions, with approximately equal numbers of participants within each misatribution con- dition assigned to the modesty norm violated (n¼21 in both misatribution conditions) and modesty norm not violated (n¼ 18 in both misatribution conditions) conditions. Because of time constraints, data from four participants (two in each of the misatribution conditions) were incomplete on the survey measures; however, because the surveys were coun- terbalanced (with the exception of the manipulation check and demographic survey which were always handed out at the very end of the study session), the missing data were not consistently on one outcome. Thus, degrees of freedom we report reflect the number of participants with complete data on that particular measure. Procedure and Materials Participants were recruited for a study on the ‘Psychology of Communication,’ supposedly in conjunction with the Ofice of Career Services, meant ‘to look at ways to improve per- sonal essays.’ Al participants were told they would be writ- ing an essay as if they were applying for a US$5,000 university scholarship and that the essays would be reviewed by a staf member and used in workshops to ‘aid undergrad- uates with their writing.’ Al participants received an actual description of a university scholarship (the ‘University Sys- tem Honor Scholarship’). This cover story and design were adapted from Moss-Racusin and Rudman (2010). At this point, al participants were told that the department was ‘researching the efects of extraneous distractions on task performance,’ and as such, everyone would complete a survey at the very end of the study on their perceptions of any distractions experienced during the session. Participants in the misatribution condition then received information about the ‘black box subliminal noise generator’ (described later). Next, al participants were instructed by the computer about whom to write (modesty norm violation manipulation): themselves (norm violation) or another student (norm not violated). Participants were given 5 min to brainstorm before the computer prompted them to begin typing their essay and then were given 20 min to complete their essay. After time elapsed, al participants completed a survey packet, were debriefed, and thanked. Participants’ essays were saved in Microsoft Word documents, and a separate set of participants rated the quality of the essays. Modesty norm violation manipulation.Al participants com- pleted the same ‘promotion task’ (scholarship essay), but the instructions for the essay difered. Participants assigned to the modesty normviolationcondition were instructed to write the essay as if they personaly were submiting it for the scholarship; specificaly, they were told to write about their own accomplishments and successes. Participants assigned to the modesty normnot violatedcondition were instructed to write the essay on behalf of a friend who was submiting for the scholarship, that is, to write about their friend’s accomplishments and successes. The use of self- versus other-directed promotion as a manipulation of the modesty norm was adapted from Moss-Racusin and Rudman (2010). Misatribution availability and manipulation check. For the manipulation of misatribution, we opted to use an external object as the source of situational arousal. (Past research has used items ranging from a cubical booth, screen filters, flick- ering lights, a pil, and a noise generator.) We selected the subliminal noise generator and created an ominous black box, replete with wires and speakers (Ben-Zeev et al., 2005; Olson, 1988). Participants in the misatribution available con- dition were told the subliminal noise generator caused side efects such as ‘increased heart rate, nervousness, and arou- sal,’ thereby providing participants with an alternate external source of any discomfort. Participants in this condition were then played sample tones by the experimenter, which in real- ity were three tones played discretely from an iPod. Partici- pants assigned to the no misatribution available condition were told nothing about the subliminal noise generator there- fore not providing them with an alternate, external source of situational arousal. The subliminal noise generator was pres- ent in the room in both conditions and in reality did not pro- duce any sounds. At the end of the study session,al participants completed a ‘departmental equipment inventory’ designed to test the effectiveness of the misatribution manipulation. To ensure that al participants could answer the item, including those not in the misatribution condition, the research assis- tant handed out a half-page survey and explained ‘now that you have completed the experiment, the primary investiga- tor and the Departmental Review Board would like to col- lect your thoughts and experiences regarding the session.’ The survey included filer itemsincluding questions about any noise they might have heard and other elements in the room (e.g., their computer) as wel as a target item about the black box generator. Participants responded to the item, ‘I feel the subliminal noise generator made me feel ner- vous,’ on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to7 (strongly agree). Motivation measures.Two sets of measures tapped partici- pants’ motivations regarding the ‘promotion task’ (essay writing). First, to assess interest, the survey measure of task ratings was adapted from Smith, Sansone, and White’s (2007) study, including 5 items (a¼.91) such as ‘I would describe this task as very interesting’ and ‘This task was fun to do.’ Al items were rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to7(strongly agree), and items were averaged so that higher scores indicated higher levels of each motivation. Second, the two performance goal subscales of the Achieve- ment Goal Questionnaire–Revised (Eliot & Murayama, 2008) were used to measure performance-avoidance goals (e.g., ‘Ijustwantedtoavoiddoingpoorlyonthistask’)and performance-approach goals (e.g., ‘My goal for the task was to do beter than most of the students’). Each item was rated from 1 (not at al true of me)to7(very true of me), and items for both the avoidance (3 items;a¼.78) and approach (3 items;a¼.87) subscales were averaged so that higher scores indicated higher levels of each motivation. Mastery goals were also assessed with 3 items but did not show any meaningful differences and are thus not reported. Performance measures.We colected two measures of parti- cipants’ performance: self-perceived performance and judges’ ratings of the quality of participants’ essays. First, embedded within a larger survey that contained 10 filer items, 4 items assessed participants’ perception of the quality of the essay they had writen. Using a scale from 1(not at al)to6 (extremely), participants rated: ‘When others read your essay, how qualified do you think they wil rate the applicant?’ ‘When others read your essay, how competent do you think they wil rate the applicant?’ ‘How wel do you think you performed on your essay today?’ and ‘How wel do you think you promoted in your essay today?’ Adapted from Moss-Racusin and Rudman (2010), the internal consistency reliability (a¼.80) of this measure was sound. Second, similar to procedures used by Moss-Racusin and Rudman (2010), a new sample of 44 introductory psychology students (60.1%women;Mage¼20.04 years; 89.7%White) served as naive raters to determine the amount of scholarship money waranted by the quality of each promotion essay. Participants were told they would be rating several personal essays writen by other students applying for a US$5,000 scholarship. Participants received a description of the scho- larship for which the essays were writen, six randomly selected essays, and six essay scholarship rating sheets. In order to mask the modesty norm violation condition, the norm not violated (other promotion) essays were changed to first person. For example, the sentence ‘She is very suc- cessful’ became ‘I am very successful.’ Participants were told to rate the essays starting at the top of the pile and to move on only after completely rating the previous essay. The essays each participant-judge received were counterbalanced to control for order efects. Judges responded to the item ‘Given a scholarship amount between $0 and $5000 how much do you think the applicant wil receive?’ Each essay writen in this study received at least two (and sometimes three) independent dolar amount judgments. Akin to a within-subjects design, with essay as the within variable and scholarship rating as the dependent vari- able, the goal was to get an average dolar amount from at least two new (naive) participants as to the scholarship amount the essay waranted. The average dolar amount awarded resulted in a data set with the 75 essays as units. No two judges rated the same set of six essays. This overal mean dolar amount served to measure the quality of each participant’s essay performance.1 Potential control measures.We also explored the possible influence of the variables on task value and effort. To measure task value, 3 items (a¼.83, e.g. ‘I think this was a valuable task’) from Smith et al.’s (2007) study were interspersedwithin the same survey as the interest items detailed above. These items were rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to7 (strongly agree) and were averaged so that higher scores indicated higher perceivedvalue of the promotion task. As an estimate of effort on the task, the number of words participants wrote for their essay was counted using a word count application. Results Preliminary Analyses The response to the item measuring the efectiveness of the misatribution manipulation was submited to a 2 (modesty norm violated vs. not violated) 2 (misatribution available vs. not available) analysis of variance (ANOVA). As expected, results yielded a significant main efect of misatri- bution condition,F(1, 74)¼21.43,p< .001,Zp2¼.23. Par- ticipants in the misatribution available condition (M¼2.81, SE¼ .17) reported that the subliminal noise generator made them feel significantly more nervous compared to the misatribution not available condition (M¼1.67,SE¼.17), indicating that the manipulation was successful. Analyses on our two exploratory variables showed no def- icits in task value among women who did and did not violate the modesty norm under normal conditions. Instead, results showed only a significant main efect of modesty norm vio- lation condition,F(1, 70)¼8.42,p¼.005,Zp2¼.11, such that across misatribution conditions, people valued the task more when self-promoting (M¼4.89,SE¼.19) compared to when other-promoting (M¼4.04,SE¼.22). We return to this point in the discussion. Analyses on the exploratory variable of word count as a possible measure of efort yielded a significant interaction between misatribution condition and modesty norm violation condition,F(1, 71)¼5.59,p¼.02, Zp2¼.14, such that women who violated the modesty norm wrote somewhat fewer words compared to women who did not violate the modesty norm,F(1, 73)¼3.84,p¼.054, Zp2¼.05. However, this diference was only marginaly sig- nificant and quite smal. Because none of these efects are in directions consistent with our hypotheses, neither task value nor word count can account for our findings and thus were not included in the tests of our hypotheses. Table 1 displays the corelations among al our measures. Because several of the motivational variables were signifi- cantly and positively corelated with each other (albeit not highly corelated), an overarching 2 (modesty norm violated vs. not violated) 2 (misatribution available vs. not avail- able) multivariate analysis of variance was conducted using the three motivational dependent measures. The predicted sig- nificant interaction emerged across the three motivation measures,F(3, 67)¼13.79,p< .001, Wilks’sL¼.62,¼.22, Zp2¼.38. As such, we felt justified in conducting separate univariate ANOVAs on each motivation measure. Hypothesis Testing For our overal analyses exploring motivation and perfor- mance, we conducted separate 2 (modesty norm violated vs. not violated) 2 (misatribution available vs. not available) ANOVAs on each measure, expecting to find Table 1.Correlations Among the Dependent Measures. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Potential control variables 1. Task value — 2. Essay word count .24* — Motivation 3. Interest .48*** .19 — 4. Performance avoidance goals .04 .07 .12 — 5. Performance approach goals .30** .22 .37*** .36** — Performance 6. Perceived performance .30** .25* .20 .02 .29* — 7. Scholarship amount awarded .19 .11 .00 .31** .03 .11 *p< .05. **p< .01. ***p< .001. significant interactions. Across misatribution conditions, no main efect of modesty norm violation condition eme- rged on the motivational variables (experience of interest, performance-avoidance goal adoption, or performance- approach goal adoption) or on the two performance variables (self-perceived performance and judged promotion quality). Instead, results yielded the predicted significant interaction between misatribution available condition and modesty norm violation condition for the experience of interest,F(1, 70)¼ 18.76,p<.001,Zp2¼.21; for performance-avoidance goal adoption,F(1, 71)¼5.22,p¼.02,Zp2¼ .07, and for performance-approach goal adoption,F(1, 71)¼ 6.29, p¼.01,Zp2¼.08. Analyses similarly yielded a significant interaction between misatribution condition and modesty norm violation condition for perceived performance,F(1, 74)¼6.84,p¼.01,Zp2¼.08, and judged promotion quality in dolars,F(1, 73)¼4.73,p¼.03,Zp2¼.06. With these omnibus test results establishing significant interactions for al three motivation measures and both performance measures in mind, we next report the results organized around our two hypotheses. We used simple effect analyses to interpret any meaningful differences across our four conditions. Hypothesis 1: Replication of the efects of modesty norm violation. Our first hypothesis focused on replicating prior findings of violating the modesty norm whereby, under normal condi- tions (i.e., no misatribution available), women would report lower motivation and perform worse when promoting the self than when promoting another person. As seen in Table 2 (under the Misatribution Unavailable column), folow-up simple efect analyses decomposing the overal significant interaction for women’s motivation previously reported showed that,when no misatribution was available, our first hypothesis was partialy confirmed. As predicted, violating the modesty norm by promoting the self resulted in women experiencing significantly less interest,F(1, 70)¼ 7.43, p¼ .008,Zp2¼ .10, and endorsing significantly more performance-avoidance goals,F(1, 71)¼7.46,p¼.008, Zp2¼.10, compared to women who did not violate the mod- esty norm. Surprisingly, under these normal conditions with no misatribution available, no significant diference emerged between groups violating the modesty norm or not on performance-approach goal adoption. Turning to women’s promotion performance, folow-up simple efect analyses showed that in line with our hypoth- esis, under normal conditions when a misatribution source was unavailable, women who violated the modesty norm per- ceived they had performed significantly worse than nonvio- lating women,F(1, 74)¼8.19,p¼.005,Zp2¼.10 (see Table 2 under the Misatribution Unavailable column). Furthermore, essays writen by self-promoting women were awarded significantly less scholarship money than essays writ- tenbyother-promotingwomen,F(1, 73)¼6.80,p¼.01, Zp2¼.09 (see Table 2). Hypothesis 2: Misatribution availability.To test our second hypothesis, we examined the significant overal interactions reported previously for evidence that when atribution of sit- uation arousal to an external source was available, the defi- ciencies we found in motivation and performance among women who violated the modesty norm under normal condi- tions would be eliminated or reversed. Now focusing on the ful 2 2 interaction, we compared women who violated the modesty norm to those who did not violate the norm when a misatribution was both unavailable and available. Table 2.Means Across Dependent Measures for Modesty Norm Misatribution Conditions. Dependent Measure Misatribution Unavailable Misatribution Available Modesty norm M SE M SE Motivation Interest Violated 2.85b .27 3.88a .28Not violated 4.03c .34 2.48b .30Performance avoidance goal adoption Violated 4.35b .20 3.56a .20Not violated 3.51a .23 3.68a .21Performance approach goal adoption Violated 3.38b .22 4.25a .22Not violated 3.69b .26 3.37b .24Performance Perceived performance Violated 3.66b .16 4.29a .17Not violated 4.37a .19 4.08a .18Judged performance Violated $1675.04b 242.53 $2611.91a 253.82Not violated $2662.60a 290.78 $2433.82a 282.10 Note. Means within each dependent measure not sharing a subscript difer atp< .05 using simple efect analyses. Hypothesis 2a: Closing the modesty norm violation gap.As shown in Table 2, folow-up simple efect analyses showed that when the subliminal noise generator was available to misatribute any arousal, violating the modesty norm (by pro- moting the self) resulted in women reporting significantly more interest for the task compared to those women who did not violate the modesty norm,F(1, 70)¼11.69,p¼.001,Zp2 ¼.14. Likewise, women who violated the norm with a mis- atribution available endorsed significantly more performance-approach goals,F(1, 71)¼7.29,p¼.009, Zp2¼.09, and adopted similar levels of performance avoid- ance goals compared to women who did not violate the mod- esty norm,F(1, 71)¼0.20,p¼.66,Zp2¼.00. Thus, for both positive forms of motivation (interest and performance approach goal adoption), having a misatribution source reversed the deficit motivation expressed by women who vio- lated the modesty norm to make self-promoting more moti- vating that other-promoting. For performance avoidance goals, the former modesty norm violation gap was closed, with both paterns for motivational measures confirming Hypothesis 2a. Analyses on performance also showed support for Hypothesis 2a by closing the modesty norm violation gap. When a misatribution source was available, the previously found diferences between self- and other-promotion on per- ceived performance and judged promotion quality were elim- inated. As shown in Table 2, women who violated the modesty norm perceived equaly good performance and received equaly high scholarship funding (see Figure 1) when a misatribution was available. These results confirm that having a misatribution available to ofset situational arousal aleviates (or even improves) the motivational and performance diferences between those who violate the mod- esty norm and those who do not violate the norm. Hypothesis 2b: Modesty norm violation and misatribution. Next, we wanted to test whether our misatribution avail- ability intervention could improve the motivation and performance for women who violated the modesty norm. Results confirmed this hypothesis. Compared to women vio- lating the modesty norm under normal conditions, women promoting the self with a misatribution available were signif- icantly more likely to express interest in the task,F(1, 70)¼ 7.03,p¼.01,Zp2¼.09, to endorse fewer performance- avoidance goals,F(1, 71)¼8.03,p¼.006,Zp2¼.10, and to adopt more performance-approach goals for the task, F(1,71)¼7.71,p¼.007,Zp2¼.10. Performance was also enhanced for women violating the modesty norm with a misatribution available, compared to when it was unavailable. As seen in Table 2, folow-up simple efect analyses showed promoting the self with a misatribution available resulted in significantly beter self- perceptions of performance compared to when no misatribu- tion was available,F(1,74)¼7.28,p¼.009,Zp2¼.09, and resulted in women receiving more scholarship money from the judges,F(1,73)¼7.12,p¼.009,Zp2¼.09. Thus, Hypothesis 2b was confirmed across al three motivational and both performance measures. Prediction 2c: Modesty norm non-violation and misatribution. Finaly, we wanted to examine whether the availability of the misatribution influenced the motivation and perfor- mance (one way or the other) for women who did not vio- late the modesty norm. Assuming that no situational arousal comes from engaging in a modesty-norm consistent task (promoting another person), we did not expect any influence of misatribution availability. As shown in Table 2, for women who did not violate the modesty norm, misatribution availability did not change the adoption of performance- avoidance goals or the adoption of performance-approach goals. Surprisingly, other-promoting women did report sig- nificantly less interest for the task when a misatribution was available (vs. not available),F(1,70)¼11.77,p¼.001, Zp2¼.14. We return to this point in the Discussion sec- tion. Performance was also largely unaffected by misatri- bution availability for women who did not violate the modesty norm. Indeed, misattribution condition did not meaningfuly change perceived performance or judged quality of the essay when the modesty norm was not vio- lated. With the exception of reported interest, the results generaly suggest that the availability of an external source to misatribute situational arousal did not help or hurt the motivation or performance of women who did not violate the modesty norm. Discussion Our results replicated and extended past work on the negative efects women experience when violating the modesty norm versus not under normal conditions (when no external source was available to atribute situational arousal). In line with Hypothesis 1, these self-promoting women experienced less interest, were more likely to adopt performance-avoidance Figure 1.The amount of scholarship money awarded in dolars by student judges (judged performance) across the four cels of the 2 2 design crossing modesty norm violation with misattribution availability. goals, had poorer perceptions of their own performance, and their essays were judged lower in quality compared to women who promoted on behalf of another. Encouragingly, results showed that we were able to close this modesty norm viola- tion gap with misatribution availability. When women who violated the modesty norm in the presence of a subliminal noise generator that they were told could be a source of dis- comfort, the motivational and performance diferences between those who violated the modesty norm or not were eliminated or even reversed. What is more, these positive efects of the misatribution availability were evident when comparing women who violated the modesty norm under nor- mal conditions with those who violated the norm with the misatribution available. The positive efects of the misatri- bution available for women who violated the modesty norm came largely without consequence to the women who did not violate the modesty norm (albeit with one exception, dis- cussed below). Taken together then, our results suggest that the availabil- ity of a misatribution source for situational arousal improved the promotion experience and the quality of promotion for women who violated the modesty norm. Importantly, our results also showed that women who violated the modesty norm and were given a misatribution arousal source reported greater interest in the promotion task compared to those not given a misatribution source. Improving women’s momen- tary self-promotion performance is important to be sure, but enhancing women’s interest is key because such intrinsic motivational experiences could potentialy result in more long-term motivation to engage in other self-promotion tasks (Csikszentmihalyi, 1978; Thoman et al., 2013). To our knowledge, ours is the first work to demonstrate that violat- ing the modesty norm impacts the experience of interest in self-promotion. Greater task interest typicaly depends on the type of achievement goals adopted during task engagement (Eliot & Harackiewicz, 1996). Indeed, our data showed that women adopted more performance-avoidance goals when self- promoting under normal conditions and adopted more performance-approach goals when self-promoting with a misatribution available. Although not examined as a med- iator (because goals were assessed folowing the promotion task), future research would benefit from understanding how performance-goal adoption, especialy performance- avoidance goals, might influence self-promotion experi- ences. We had predicted that women who violated the modesty norm under normal conditions would adopt performance-approach goals less often, but this was not the case. Perhaps the nature of thetask instructions (to write an essay to try to win a scholarship) subtly triggered performance-approach goals to‘dowelcomparedtooth- ers’ among al participants, washing out the effects. Never- theless, when a misatribution was available, performance approach goals were adopted more often by women who violated the modesty norm compared to those who did not violate the norm suggesting that these goals are important to consider. As laid out at the onset, the act of self-promotion is a vio- lation of the modesty norm. Thus, we explored the possibility that task value and efort (as indexed by number of words writen) would also be negatively impacted when violating the modesty norm. Yet, under normal conditions (i.e., when no misatribution source was available), the perceived value of the task remained the same no mater if the modesty norm was violated or not. Moreover, colapsing across the two mis- atribution conditions, women who promoted the self rated the task higher in value compared to women who promoted other people. It is possible that the nature of the task (a scho- larship essay) is seen as more personaly valuable generaly speaking because of the potential for direct financial gain. Certainly, decades of work on the ‘self-referencing’ efect and ‘ego-involving’ task orientations suggest that self- related tasks are often processed at a deeper level and are viewed as more important and interesting (Butler, 1987; Symons & Johnson, 1997). Stil, despite seeing the higher value of the self-promotion task, these women were less likely to enjoy the task and sufered performance decrements compared to women who did not violate the modesty norm under normal conditions. Furthermore, our results showed some negative efects of the subliminal noise generator such that women who other- promoted when the misatribution source was available reported less task interest compared to women who other- promoted without a misatribution available. It was predicted that other-promoting would not be afected by the availability of a misatribution because other-promoting does not violate the modesty norm. One possibility for this finding is that the misatribution source muted any positive feelings people might have when writing about the accomplishments of a friend, decreasing any ‘basking in reflected glory’ afiliation experiences (Snyder, Lassegard, & Ford, 1986). However, we interpret these results with caution, given that no other mea- sure showed these unexpected results. Potential Sources of Situational Arousal Taken together, results of the current project showed that violating the modesty norm can act as a barrier to women’s self-promotion and that, under normal circumstances (when no misatribution source for any discomfort is provided), self-promotion is of lower perceived and actual quality; results in greater performance-avoidance goal adoption; and diminishes the experienceof interest. However, when women self-promote under conditions that provide an opportunity to misatribute anysituational arousal, the gap between violating the modesty norm and not violating the modesty norm was eliminated (to similar levels of performance-avoidance goal adoption as wel as perceived and actual performance) or even reversed (higher interest and performance-approach goal adoption). This evidence suggests that, under particular situational arousal-reducing circumstances, women are able to promote as wel for them- selves as they do for others. There are a number of possible explanations for these find- ings. For example, decades of research on Festinger’s cogni- tive dissonance theory shows that when people behave in a manner inconsistent with their own beliefs, they experience discomfort (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959; see also Cooper et al., 1978; Eliot & Devine, 1994; Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, Simon, & Nelson, 1996). Cognitive dissonance occurs any time a person has two psychologicaly conflicting cognitions, such as behaving in a manner inconsistent with opinions, or as we argue here, when behaving in a manner inconsistent with gender norms. Thus, women in our study may have experienced arousal because they were asked to self-promote and such behavior is inconsistent with the mod- esty norm. Another possible explanation is that women who self- promote experience fear-of-backlash (for a review, see Rudman et al., 2012), and this backlash fear manifests as situational arousal. As outlined in the backlash avoidance model (Moss-Racusin & Rudman, 2010), women experience wory and fear over the (often very real) ‘social and eco- nomic penalties’ associated with violating gender norms (Brescol, 2011; Rudman, 1998). This fear of backlash may manifest as situational arousal, although this connec- tion remains for future research to document. Worry or fear over confirming a stereotype is yet another alternative explanation for the source of situational arousal experi- enced by women who self-promoted in our study. Women who worry about confirming a stereotype, ironicaly, underperform and experience reduced task motivation (for reviews, see Nguyen & Ryan, 2008; Thoman et al., 2013). Although the role of anxiety in stereotype threat effects on performance is uncertain (see Smith, 2004, for a review), there is converging evidence that stereotype threat creates generalized situational arousal (Johns et al., 2005; O’Brien & Crandal, 2003) that may manifest as physiological arousal (Croizet et al., 2004). Thus, to the extent that women who self-promoted were concerned about confirming gender stereotypes, stereotype threat may have triggered situational arousal. Our curent data cannot speak to the nature of the situational arousal, but no mater the explanation (e.g., cog- nitive dissonance, fear of backlash, and stereotype threat), our data show when a source for any uncomfortable arousal was provided, this misatribution resulted in increased pro- motion success. Practice Implications Our results add to the literature that should give search com- mitees and award commitees pause in relying heavily on self-generated cover leters and essays in making determina- tions of merit because women may undersel themselves fol- lowing societal norms associated with modesty gender norms (Moss-Racusin & Rudman, 2010). One strength of our proj- ect was in testing misatribution as an intervention; we found the availability of an object (in our case, a subliminal noise generator) to misatribute any situational discomfort closed the gap between those women who did and did not violate the modesty norm. Admitedly, a fake subliminal noise generator would be dificult to implement in practice. However, teling people that their computer screen or the cubical in which they are working can cause discomfort are other possible external objects that might be used to atribute situational arousal. The next step is to feret out the short- and long-term benefits of these and other misatribution strategies. For example, women who are taught to blame gender stereotypes for any discomfort they experience during a math test perform beter (Johns et al., 2005). Perhaps women who are taught to blame modesty norms would similarly show an improvement in self-promotion. Along these lines, ‘normalizing’ self- promotion as ‘part of the job’ (particularly in certain domains that are male-dominated, such as science and math) could result in an improvement not only to a woman’s self- promotion experience but also her feelings of belonging and motivation for the job itself (for a similar ‘normalizing’ intervention strategy regarding efort expenditures, see Smith, Lewis, Hawthorne, & Hodges, 2013). Limitations and Next Steps It is certainly possible that al people, including men, experi- ence situational arousal when promoting the self. Although our focus here was on women, future research wil benefit from such a gender comparison. However, the modesty norm is particular to women’s gender roles (Bronstein, 2006); because men’s gender roles include traits such as arogance and assertiveness (Moss-Racusin et al., 2010), thus, it is unlikely that the modesty norm would similarly limit men’s self-promotion. Nevertheless, men who do violate gender norms associated with agency experience backlash by others. For example, when a male candidate answered interview questions in a modest manner, he sufered interpersonal con- sequences (by observers) for violating the gender norm that men should be agentic (Moss-Racusin et al., 2010). Future research would also do wel to include cross-cultural compar- isons to determine the generalizability of the efects of violat- ing the modesty norm (and ways to ofset it) because, in some cultures (e.g., Japan), men and women alike are expected to be modest and humble (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). To our knowledge, our research is the first to consider women’s self-promotion motivational experiences and per- formance tendencies using a misatribution paradigm. Past work assumed women were capable of self-promotion because of their ability to promote another person (Moss-Racusin & Rudman, 2010); thus, the deficiency is assumed to not be in promotion ability per se. Our data are the first known to clearly demonstrate that women’s self-promotion can be just as high in quality as other-promotion, and, what is more, that the promotion experience can be positive. When provided with a source to misatribute discomfort they might experience while self-promoting, women reported enhanced motivation and per- ceived their performance to be beter, and their essays were awarded more scholarship money by independent judges. This is important considering that women must self-promote in order to acquire educational and career advancement. The gender wage gap, for example, persists, and it is possible that the uncomfortable arousal women experience when self- promoting is contributing to this gap. If self-promotion is nec- essary and women are uncomfortable doing so then, without a situational intervention, their applications may be viewed as lower in quality and thus women may be passed over for pro- motions and raises. Our data ofer insight into one situational option for helping women self-promote to advance in their pro- fessions or negotiate beter wages or start-up packages. It is imperative that, as scholars identify ways to help women self-promote, there is a simultaneous efort to combat the very real backlash that self-promoting women face (Bowles & Babcock, 2012; Brescol, 2011; Moss-Racusin et al., 2010; Rudman et al., 2012). Perhaps there is a threshold for a ‘proper’ amount of self-promotion that would both ben- efit woman’s educational and career success and not result in social or economic sanctions. Or perhaps there are ways to standardized self-promotion tasks (e.g., with clear question/ answer options) or other ways to gain the same information (e.g., require leters of reference instead of self-generated let- ters—albeit leters of reference can fal subject to gender bias as wel; Schmader, Whitehead, & Wysocki, 2007). Indeed, research on reframing task instructions in a way that is mod- esty norm consistent (e.g., as an opportunity to ‘politely ask’ versus ‘negotiate’) enhances the likelihood women wil initiate a negotiation (Smal et al., 2007). Conclusion The curent findings suggest that women experience uncom- fortable arousal when promoting the self because such self- promotion violates gender norms for modesty. American reli- gious leader Howard Hunter (1997, p. 123) once stated in his teachings that:‘The girl who chooses to be modest chooses to be respected. A boy who is honest with himself wil admit that he likes a girl who is modest in speech, conduct, and dress. Modesty is one of the great virtues.’ Such messages about gender and modesty are conveyed in many ways and are ingrained in American culture (Bronstein, 2006; Budworth & Mann, 2010; Rudman, 1998). We demonstrated that, without providing for the misatribution of situational arousal (e.g., discomfort) that resulted from violation of the modesty norm, women may resolve their discomfort by being modest and downplaying their own accomplishments. With a situational intervention, however, women can misatribute the source of situational arousal and are then free to promote themselves. Indeed, bragging ought to be a ‘right’ aforded to everyone. Acknowledgment We are grateful to the research assistants in the Motivation and Diversity Lab for their assistance with data colection. Authors’ Notes Parts of study served as the second author’s Women and Gender Studies Thesis. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the folowing financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study was supported in part by a Montana State University Undergraduate Scholars Program Grant. Note 1. This methodology was borowed from Moss-Racusin and Rud- man (2010, study 2), with the only exception that we masked the self- versus other-promotion nature of the condition. Among the 44 new participant ‘scholarship judges,’ because no two partici- pants ever rated the same set of six essays, it is not appropriate to compute interater reliability. The goal was to get an average rat- ing from at least two new participants regarding the scholarship award money the essay waranted, similar to how an actual panel of judges might review applications for a scholarship. This pro- cedure is akin to a repeated measures design with essay as the within-subjects variable and scholarship rating as the dependent variable. References Alen, J., Gervais, S. J., & Smith, J. L. (2013). Sit big to eat big: The interaction of body posture and body concern on restrained eat- ing.Psychology of Women Quarterly,37, 325–336. Alen, J., & Smith, J. L. (2011). The influence of sexuality stereotypes on men’s experience of gender-role incongruence. Psychology of Men & Masculinity,12, 77–96. doi:10.1037/ a0019678 Amanatulah, E. T., & Moris, M. W. (2010). Negotiating gender roles: Gender diferences in assertive negotiating are mediated by women’s fear of backlash and atenuated when negotiating on behalf of others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy,98, 256–267. doi:10.1037/a0017094 Ben-Zeev, T., Fein, S., & Inzlicht, M. (2005). Arousal and stereo- type threat.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,41, 174–181. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.007 Blakemore, J. E. O. (2003). Children’s beliefs about violating gender norms: Boys shouldn’t look like girls, and girls shouldn’t act like boys.Sex Roles,48, 411–419. doi:10.1023/A:1023574427720 Boiche, J., Plaza, M., Chalabaev, A., Guilet, E., & Sarazin, P. (2014). Social antecedents and consequences of sport gender stereotypes during adolescence.Psychology of Women Quar- terly,38(2), 259–274. doi:10.1177/0361684313505844 Bowles, H. R., & Babcock, L. (2012). How can women escape the compensation negotiation dilemma? Relational accounts are one answer.Psychology of Women Quarterly,37, 80–96. doi:10. 1177/0361684312455524 Brescol, V. L. (2011). Who takes the floor and why: Gender, power and volubility in organizations.Administrative Science Quar- terly,56, 622–641. doi:10.1177/0001839212439994 Brescol, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2008). Can an angry woman get ahead? Status conferal, gender, and expression of emotion in the workplace.Psychological Science,19, 268–275. doi:10.1111/j. 1467-9280.2008.02079.x Brodish, A. B., & Devine, P. G. (2009). The role of performance- avoidance goals and wory in mediating the relationship between stereotype threat and performance.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,45, 180–185. Bronstein, P. (2006).The family environment: Where gender role socialization begins. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Budworth, M., & Mann, S. L. (2010). Becoming a leader: The chal- lenge of modesty for women.Journal of Management Develop- ment,29, 177–186. doi:10.1108/02621711011019314 Butler, R. (1987). Task involving and ego involving properties of eva- luation: Efects of diferent feedback conditions on motivational perceptions, interest, and performance.Journal of Educational Psychology,79, 474–482. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.474 Condry, J., & Condry, S. (1976). Sex differences: A study of the eye of the beholder.Child Development,47,812–819. Retrieved from htp:/search.proquest.com/docview/616116482? accountid¼28148 Cooper, J. (1998). Unlearning cognitive dissonance: Toward an understanding of the development of dissonance.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,34, 562–575. doi:10.1006/ jesp.1998.1365 Cooper, J., Zanna, M. P., & Taves, P. A. (1978). Arousal as a neces- sary condition for atitude change folowing induced compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,36, 1101–1106. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.10.1101 Crawford, M. (1988). Gender, age, and the social evaluation of assertion.Behavior Modification,12, 549–564. doi:10.1177/ 01454455880124004 Croizet, J., Despre´s, G., Gauzins, M., Huguet, P., Leyens, J., & Me´ot, A. (2004). Stereotype threat undermines intelectual per- formance by triggering a disruptive mental load.Personality and Social Psychology Buletin,30, 721–731. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Intrinsic rewards and emergent moti- vation. In M. R. Lepper & D. Greene (Eds.),The hidden costs of reward(pp. 205–216). Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Diekman, A. B., Brown, E. R., Johnston, A. M., & Clark, E. K. (2010). Seeking congruity between goals and roles: A new look at why women opt out of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics careers. Psychological Science,21, 1051–1057. doi:10.1177/0956797610377342 Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex diferences and similarities: A curent appraisal. In T. Eckes &H.M.Trautner(Eds.),The developmental social psychology of gender(pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Eliot, A. J., & Devine, P. G. (1994). On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,67, 382–394. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.382 Eliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoid- ance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 461–475. Eliot, A., & McGregor, H. (2001). A 2 2 achievement goal frame- work.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,80, 501–519. Eliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, ilustration, and application.Jour- nal of Educational Psychology,100, 613–628. doi:10.1037/ 0022-0663.100.3.613 Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance.The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- chology,58, 203–210. doi:10.1037/h0041593 Finney, S. J., Pieper, S. L., & Baron, K. E. (2004). Examining the psychometric properties of the Achievement Goal Questionnaire in a general academic context.Educational and Psychological Measurement,64, 365–383. Gould, R. J., & Slone, C. G. (1982). The ‘feminine modesty’ efect: A self-presentational interpretation of sex diferences in causal atribution.Personality and Social Psychology Buletin,8, 477–485. doi:10.1177/0146167282083014 Harackiewicz, J., Baron, K., Tauer, J., Carter, S., & Eliot, A. (2000). Short-term and long-term consequences of achievement goals: Pre- dicting interest and performance over time.Journal of Educational Psychology,92, 316–330. doi:10.1037/0022-0663,92.2.316 Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., & Nel- son, D. E. (1996). Evidence that the production of aversive con- sequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 5–16. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.5 Horgan, T., & Smith, J. L. (2006). Interpersonal reasons for interper- sonal perceptions: Gender-incongruent purpose goals and non- verbal judgment accuracy.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,30, 127–140. doi:10.1007/s10919-006-0012-4 Hunter, H. W. (Ed.). (1997).The teachings of Howard W. Hunter. Salt Lake City, UT: Bookcraft. Inzlicht, M., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2000). A threatening intelectual envi- ronment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem- solving deficits in the presence of males.Psychological Science, 11, 365–371. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00272 Johns,M.,Schmader,T.,&Martens,A.(2005).Knowingishalf the batle: Teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women’s math performance. Psychological Science,16, 175–179. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00799.x Leaper, C., Breed, L., Hofman, L., & Perlman, C. A. (2002). Var- iations in the gender-stereotyped content of children’s television cartoons across genres.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1653–1662. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02767.x Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Impli- cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation.Psychological Review,98, 224–253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 Moss-Racusin, C., Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). When men break the gender rules: Status incongruity and backlash against modest men.Psychology of Men & Masculinity,11, 140–151. doi:10.1037/a0018093 Moss-Racusin, C., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). Disruptions in women’s self-promotion: The backlash avoidance model. Psychology of Women Quarterly,34, 186–202. doi:10.1111/j. 1471-6402.2010.01561.x Nguyen, H.-H. H., & Ryan, A. M. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat afect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence.Journal of Applied Psychology,93, 1314–1334. doi:10.1037/a0012702 O’Brien, L. T., & Crandal, C. S. (2003). Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women’s math performance.Personality and Social Psychology Buletin,29, 782–789. doi:10.1177/ 0146167203029006010 Olson, J. M. (1988). Misatribution, preparatory information, and speech anxiety.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 758–767. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.758 Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression man- agement.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74, 629–645. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629 Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers.Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology,77, 1004–1010. doi:10.1037/ 0022-3514.77.5.1004 Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women.Journal of Social Issues, 57, 743–762. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00239 Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Glick, P., & Phelan, J. E. (2012). Reactions to vanguards: Advances in backlash theory. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol. 45, pp. 167–227). New York, NY: Elsevier. Schmader, T., Whitehead, J., & Wysocki, V. H. (2007). A linguistic comparison of leters of recommendation for male and female chemistry and biochemistry job applicants.Sex Roles,57, 509–514. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9291-4 Shaw, J. I., & Skolnick, P. (1973). An investigation of relative pre- ference for consistency motivation.European Journal of Social Psychology,3, 271–280. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420030306 Smal, D. A., Gelfand, M., Babcock, L., & Getman, H. (2007). Who goes to the bargaining table? The influence of gender and framing on the initiation of negotiation.Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology,4, 600–613. doi:10.1037/ 0022-3514.93.4.600 Smith, J. L. (2004). Understanding the process of stereotype threat: A review of mediational variables and new performance goal directions.Educational Psychology Review,16, 177–206. doi: 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000034020.20317.89 Smith,J.L.,Lewis,K.L.,Hawthorne,L.,&Hodges,S.D.(2013). When trying hard isn’t natural: Women’s belonging with and motivation for male-dominated STEM fields as a function of effort expenditure concerns.Personality and Social Psychology Buletin,39, 3–15. doi:10.1177/0146167212468332 Smith, J. L., Sansone, C., & White, P. H. (2007). The stereotyped task engagement process: The role of interest and achievement motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology,99, 99–114. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.99 Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nulified stereotypes on math- ematical performance: It’s not just a woman’s issue.Sex Roles, 47, 179–191. Snyder, C. R., Lassegard, M., & Ford, C. E. (1986). Distancing after group success and failure: Basking in reflected glory and cuting of reflected failure.Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy,51, 382–388. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.2.382 Symons, C., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference efect in memory: A meta analysis.Psychological Buletin,12, 371–394. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371 Thoman, D. B., Smith, J. L., Brown, E. R., Chase, J., & Lee, J. Y. K. (2013). Beyond performance: A motivational experiences model of stereotype threat.Educational Psychology Review,25, 211–243. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9219-1 Urdan, T. (Ed.). (1999).Advances in motivation and achievement: Motivation in context(Vol. 11). Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Wade, M. E. (2001). Women and the salary negotiation: The costs of self-advocacy.Psychology of Women Quarterly,25, 65–76. doi: 10.1111/1471-6402.00008 Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). Dissonance and the pil: An atri- bution approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,29, 703–709. doi: 10.1037/h0036651