Situational and Dispositional Factors as Antecedents of Ingratiatory Behaviors in Organizational Settings Authors: K. Michele Kacmar, Dawn S. Carlson, and Virginia K. Bratton NOTICE: this is the author’s version of a work that was accepted for publication in Journal of Vocational Behavior. Changes resulting from the publishing process, such as peer review, editing, corrections, structural formatting, and other quality control mechanisms may not be reflected in this document. Changes may have been made to this work since it was submitted for publication. A definitive version was subsequently published in Journal of Vocational Behavior, [VOL# 65, ISSUE# 2, (October 2004)] DOI# 10.1016/j.jvb.2003.09.002 Kacmar, K. Michele, Dawn S. Carlson, and Virginia K. Bratton. “Situational and Dispositional Factors as Antecedents of Ingratiatory Behaviors in Organizational Settings.” Journal of Vocational Behavior 65, no. 2 (October 2004): 309–331. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2003.09.002. Made available through Montana State University’s ScholarWorks scholarworks.montana.edu Situational and dispositional factors as antecedents of ingratiatory behaviors AbstractThis study examined both situational and dispositional antecedents of four ingratiatory be- haviors: other-enhancing, opinion conformity, favor rendering, and self-promotion. The two situational variables (i.e., role ambiguity and leader-member exchange) and the four disposi- tional variables (i.e., self-esteem, need for power, job involvement, and shyness) were considered as antecedents to each of the ingratiatory behaviors. Results from a sample of 136 full-time employees suggested that each of the ingratiatory behaviors had a unique set of antecedents and that the dispositional variables explained significant additional variance beyond the variance explained by the situational variables.in organizational settings K. Michele Kacmar,a Dawn S. Carlson,b and Virginia K. Brattona a Department of Management, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306-1110, USA b Hankamer School of Business, Baylor University, P.O. Box 98006 Waco, TX 76798-8006, USA 1. Introduction Impression management in organizational settings has been studied since Goffman (1959) introduced his dramaturgical interpretation of social interaction. While many impression management tactics have been identified and studied since this time, one in particular, ingratiation, has generated the most research attention (Gordon, 1996). Ingratiation has been broadly defined as an attempt by individuals to increase their attractiveness in the eyes of others (Wortman & Linsenmeier, 1977). Specifically, in- dividuals enact ingratiation tactics to elicit an attribution of competence or likeability (Jones & Pittman, 1982). To accomplish this goal, a variety of ingratiation tactics can be employed such as the other-focused tactics of other-enhancement, opinion confor- mity, favor rendering, and the self-focused tactic of self-promotion (Jones, 1964; Ku- mar & Beyerlein, 1991). While a great deal of research has examined the outcomes of ingratiation under the broader umbrella of impression management, much less has explored what drives people to engage in ingratiatory behaviors. It is clear from examining models and reviews of ingratiation and more generally, impression management, that two of the major antecedents to these behaviors are the situation in which the ingratiator finds himself or herself and his or her disposition (Bozeman & Kacmar, 1997; Gilmore, Stevens, Harrell-Cook, & Ferris, 1999; Kris- tof-Brown, Barrick, & Franke, 2002; Liden & Mitchell, 1988). While advocates of the middle ground perspective would argue that both domains (situational and dis- positional) contribute to our understanding and have to be considered simulta- neously (Newton & Keenan, 1991; Schneider, 1987; Staw & Ross, 1985), few studies have empirically tested multiple elements of both of these antecedents in a single study. Given the expected importance of these antecedents to the ingratiation process, considering both situational and dispositional variables as simultaneous predictors of ingratiatory behaviors should provide more insight into the ingratia- tion process than studying each separately (Ralston, 1985). Coupling this notion with prior prescriptions that greater emphasis be placed on investigating the per- son-situation debate (Borman, White, & Hedge, 1997; Hattrup & Jackson, 1996), we propose that approaching the study of ingratiation from the combined disposi- tional–situational perspective may, in fact, be highly appropriate (Newton & Kee- nan, 1991; Schneider, 1987; Staw & Ross, 1985). Similar to Chatmans (1989) model of person-organization fit, in the current study we analyze an additive model that incorporates the influence of both situational and dispositional factors on ingra- tiation behaviors in organizational settings. Role theory, which explains organizational behavior by examining the nature of individual roles within organizations and the processes by which these roles are de- fined and developed (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Graen, 1976), can offer an explanation as to how both situational and dispositional influences affect ones decision to use ingratiation. Roles and role behaviors are developed through a set of processes that are dependent not only upon the individuals engaged in these processes, but situa- tional influences as well. Role theory suggests that individuals who use ingratiation do so in an effort to shape the evolution of their roles and that these actions are con-tingent upon events leading up to the decision to engage in such behavior (situational ingratiation. Such information can be useful in a number of ways. For example, past studies have yielded conflicting findings regarding dispositional antecedents of ingra-tiation (Gilmore et al., 1999; Kristof-Brown et al., 2002). While it is intuitive to ex- pect positive relationships between certain personality variables (such as locus of control, Machiavellianism, self-monitoring, and self-esteem) and IM tactic use, some studies have found no relationship to exist (Anderson, Silverster, Cunningham-Snell, & Haddleton, 1999; Delery & Kacmar, 1998) where other studies have provided sup- port for these relationships (Appelbaum & Hughes, 1998; Delery & Kacmar, 1998; Fletcher, 1990). Offering additional evidence about the relationships among certain dispositional variables and ingratiation may help clear up this confusion. The second major contribution offered by this study is the simultaneous examination of the in- fluences of situational and dispositional variables in the ingratiation process. As pre- vious studies rarely incorporated both situational and dispositional variables in their investigations of ingratiation, the results yielded by such studies are likely to be in- complete. Thus, by taking a comprehensive approach, this study may help to clarify our understanding of the ingratiation process. Toward this end, the present study, under the theoretical umbrella of role theory, investigated the influence of the situational variables of role ambiguity and leader- member exchange (LMX) and the dispositional variables of self-esteem, need for power, job involvement, and shyness, on the use of the ingratiatory behaviors of other-enhancing, opinion conformity, favor rendering, and self-promotion. Following a combined dispositional–situational approach, the additional contribution made by dispositional factors beyond situational factors in determining the use of ingratiation tactics will be considered to see if this more encompassing approach merits attention. 2. Impression management and ingratiatory behaviors Impression management has been defined as the process through which individu- als seek to influence the image others have of them (Rosenfeld, Giacalone, & Rior- dan, 1995). The manipulation of ones image is accomplished by enacting impression management tactics. The tactics can be used to highlight the positive qualities of the individual or to focus the conversation on the other person. Most scholars agree that ingratiation is one particular form of impression management (Liden & Mitchell, 1988). In fact, several studies of impression management operationalized this con-influences) as well as the characteristics of individual actors (dispositional influences) that predispose them to perform ingratiation (Scandura, Graen, & Novak, 1986). Gi- ven the potential contributions of role theory as a framework for analyzing anteced- ents to ingratiation, we seek to enhance existing knowledge of ingratiation by investigating its antecedents from a combined dispositional–situational perspective and further, by using role theory as a guide in our selection of the antecedent vari- ables in this investigation. This paper offers two contributions to the existing literature on ingratiation. First, it provides additional insights into the driving forces behind an individuals use ofstruct using measures of ingratiation (i.e., Bolino & Turnley, 2003; Ellis, West, Ryan, & DeShon, 2002; Kristof-Brown et al., 2002; Turnley & Bolino, 2001; Wayne & Li- den, 1995). As past research has focused on similar antecedents for both impression management and ingratiation, and because ingratiation is a form of impression man- agement, extant theory and research efforts in both of these areas will be drawn upon to develop our hypotheses. 2.1. Ingratiation Ingratiation, according to Tedeschi and Melburg (1984), refers to a set of assertive tactics that are enacted by individuals to gain the approbation of superiors who con- trol significant rewards for them in organizational settings. There are two main goals of ingratiation. The first is to be liked which is accomplished through other-focused ingratiation tactics and the second is to appear competent which is achieved through self-focused ingratiation tactics (Jones & Pittman, 1982). Based on the work of Jones (1964) and Jones and Wortman (1973) four dimensions of ingratiation have been identified, three other-focused (other-enhancement, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) and one self-focused (self-promotion). Other-enhancements include flattery, verbal praises, or expressing an interest in the targets personal life in order to increase the attraction between the target and actor (Deluga & Perry, 1994; Stevens & Kristof, 1995; Wayne & Kacmar, 1991). The in- gratiator finds ways to express a positive evaluation of the target to convey the im- pression that he or she is highly regarded. When an ingratiator openly agrees with the target or expresses opinions that are similar to those held by the target, he or she is employing opinion conformity (Kumar & Beyerlein, 1991; Liden & Mitchell, 1988; Ralston & Elsass, 1989). These behaviors range from simple agreement with opinions to more elaborate attempts at trying to articulate the position held by the target as though it were ones own. Favor rendering, as the name suggests, occurs when an actor offers or actually per- forms a nonrequired task for the target. This action creates a debt owed by the target due to the norm of reciprocity (Deluga & Perry, 1991) and increases the affect the target has for the actor (Jones & Pittman, 1982; Wayne & Kacmar, 1991; Wortman & Linsenmeier, 1977). The category of self-promotion has been considered a self-focused form of ingra- tiation (Jones, 1964; Liden & Mitchell, 1988). Several behaviors would fall under this category such as: self-description, describing oneself in a manner that enhances ones attractiveness (Jones, 1964); self-presentation, making verbal statements about ones attributes to increase the likelihood of being attractive to the target (Deluga & Perry, 1994; Kumar & Beyerlein, 1991; Wayne & Liden, 1995); and self-enhancing, commu- nications intended to persuade the target of the actors positive qualities, traits, mo- tives, or intentions (Wayne & Ferris, 1990; Wayne & Kacmar, 1991). Research has clearly shown the benefit of using ingratiatory behaviors to secure the attribution of competence (self-focused) or increase affect (other-focused). For example, ingratiation has been found to increase performance appraisal ratings (Wayne & Kacmar, 1991), interview offers (Kacmar, Delery, & Ferris, 1992), andthe quality of the supervisor-subordinate relationship (Deluga & Perry, 1991; Wayne positional variables—self-esteem, need for power, job involvement, and shy- ness—that warranted inclusion in our study. Role ambiguity and LMX are centralvariables in the current analysis for they are not only important contextual influences in developing work roles, they also have been identified as key correlates of impres- sion management (Wayne & Liden, 1995). With respect to the dispositional variables, it seems intuitively sound to consider the dispositional trait, need for power, as an antecedent to ingratiation because one can establish or enhance a power base by enacting ingratiation tactics (Ralston & El-& Ferris, 1990; Wayne & Liden, 1995). What is less clear are the antecedents that drive an actor to exhibit impression management behaviors, and more specifically in- gratiation tactics. We believe the answer to this question can be found by examining both situational and dispositional antecedents of ingratiation. The tenants of role theory supply the underlying theoretical rationale for selecting the situational and dispositional variables examined in this study. 3. Role theory As role theory suggests, organizational members accomplish their work through enacting roles. Individuals learn their roles and how to successfully enact them through a series of interactions and exchanges with others in the organization (Graen, 1976). Because others in the organization have vested interests in the role performance of those around them, during these exchanges they exert pressure on the role incumbents in the form of role expectations in accordance with their self-in- terests (Dienesch & Liden, 1986). Role theory has been used in the past to explain the influence of ingratiation on several organizational outcomes such as leader-member exchange and performance ratings (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Wayne & Liden, 1995). Past research also has iden- tified impression management as an important consequence of processes in which or- ganizational roles are defined (Wayne & Green, 1993). In addition, role theory has been used to theoretically explain the importance of a number of situational and in- dividual variables (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Liden, Sparrowe, & Wayne, 1997). For example, researchers have examined its relationship with numerous contextual vari- ables such as work group composition, leader power, organizational policies, and or- ganizational culture (Dienesch & Liden, 1986). Past studies also have established links between several dispositional and demographics traits and role defining pro- cesses which include gender (Duchon, Green, & Tabor, 1986), locus of control (Kin- icki & Vecchio, 1994; Phillips & Bedeian, 1994), and positive/negative affectivity (Day & Crain, 1992). Thus, by using role theory to frame the current investigation, we have both theoretical and empirical justification to investigate numerous situa- tional and dispositional variables that may explain why individuals will be more likely to use ingratiatory behaviors when developing their role behaviors. By combining role theory with impression management theory, we were able to identify two key contextual variables—role ambiguity and LMX—and four key dis-sass, 1989). Moreover, past studies have suggested that individuals who ingratiate do the expectations for fulfilling that role have been pre-established and should be read- ily available. If, however, the position is new to the organization, information aboutthe expectations for the role may not be forthcoming (Graen, 1976). Further, it may be that the individual was hired to ‘‘recreate’’ the role, making role expectations un- clear. In equivocal situations such as these, negative consequences can occur. One such negative outcome is role ambiguity. Role ambiguity is the lack of necessary information available about a given orga- nizational position or the lack of role clarity (Kahn et al., 1964). Several models of ingratiation and impression management include role ambiguity as a situational pre- dictor variable (e.g., Gilmore et al., 1999; Liden & Mitchell, 1988). Ralston (1985), applying an expectancy theory framework, suggested that when role ambiguity was high individuals would be more inclined to use ingratiation strategies as a means of ensuring rewards they value. He attributed the rise in ingratiatory behaviors to theso in order to build or protect their self-esteem (Liden & Mitchell, 1988). We also ex- pect that the extent to which an employee is involved in his/her job will be crucial in shaping role expectations. Furthermore, it will be interesting to determine whether ‘‘living, eating, and breathing’’ ones work (i.e., job involvement) will deter an individ- uals proclivity to engage in ingratiation. However, a dispositional variable that is rarely studied as an antecedent to ingratiation in business research is shyness. Al- though shyness has been linked to impression management in psychology (Leary & Buckley, 2001; Leary & Schlenker, 1981; Schlenker & Leary, 1985), this trait is rarely examined in business management literature andmay play a significant role in whether an individual engages in ingratiatory behaviors. In the following sections we examine each of these antecedents in more detail beginning with the situational variables. 4. Situational antecedents The situational perspective of behavior suggests that changes in the work environ- ment will lead to changes in job attitudes and affective responses (Mischel, 1968). Be- cause the situation may lend itself to certain behaviors depending on context, researchers believe situational characteristics to be important in influencing behaviors (e.g., Eder & Buckley, 1988; Ferris & Judge, 1991). The situational perspective is il- lustrated in most models of impression management by including a direct path from situational factors to the use of impression management tactics (Bozeman & Kacmar, 1997; Gilmore et al., 1999; Liden & Mitchell, 1988). In the sections that follow, we apply role theory in an effort to explain how the two situational factors of role ambi- guity and leader-member exchange, may influence the use of ingratiatory behavior. 4.1. Role ambiguity Work is accomplished in organizations by individuals fulfilling roles. For each role in an organization there are expectations associated with it (Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, & Rosenthal, 1964). If the person hired is taking over an existing role,ambiguous environment blurring the link between task completion and obtaining goals. Individuals will turn to ingratiation because they are unwilling to rely on their own judgment of whether they have completed the ill-defined task well enough to earn a reward. Since the ambiguity is related to the task at hand, individuals in a highly ambiguous work environment may engage in self-focused ingratiation tactics (self-promotion) in order to ensure the perception of task competence and position themselves to receive valued rewards (Ferris & Judge, 1991). However, in environ- ments of low role ambiguity, the performance—reward link is clear, allowing indi- viduals to have some confidence in their ability to successfully perform their tasks. Secure in the fact that they have performed well enough to receive a reward, individ- uals will be more likely to turn their attention to other focused tactics as a means to further influence their superiors and gain additional rewards. Therefore, we expect that individuals will be likely to employ other-focused ingratiation tactics in environ- ments defined by low role ambiguity. Therefore, we predict Hypothesis 1a. Low role ambiguity will be positively related to other-focused (other- enhancements, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) ingratiatory behaviors. Hypothesis 1b. High role ambiguity will be positively related to self-focused (self- promotion) ingratiatory behaviors. 4.2. Leader-member exchange A second situational variable considered to influence the engagement of ingratia- tory behaviors is leader-member exchange (LMX) (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Graen & Scandura, 1987). The roots of LMX also can be traced to role theory. LMX theory posits that subordinates enact various roles in their jobs, and their supervisors have a direct influence on which roles they fulfill. Individuals selected for the more impor- tant roles share a close relationship with their supervisor and enjoy a high quality LMX relationship while the remaining subordinates are considered to have a more distant, low quality LMX relationship (Liden et al., 1997). Subordinates in a high quality LMX relationship experience a more positive relationship with the supervisor that includes a high level of trust, frequent interaction, strong support, and more re- wards, both formal and informal, than do subordinates in low quality LMX rela- tionships (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Graen & Scandura, 1987). Although ingratiation has most frequently been examined as an antecedent to LMX relationships (e.g., Dienesch & Liden, 1986), there is evidence that these con- structs can exist in a symbiotic relationship. Because of the focus of past research on LMX as an antecedent to ingratiation, it may appear that once individuals have de- veloped a high quality LMX relationship with their supervisors they no longer put forth effort ingratiating them. However, this is not necessarily the case. As role the- ory suggests, upon establishing mutual role expectations individuals may feel pres- sure to maintain their role behaviors in order to preserve their desired status. In other words, the type of working relationship that exists between a subordinate and a supervisor should influence the type of ingratiation behaviors employed (De-luga & Perry, 1991; Dockery & Steiner, 1990; Wayne & Green, 1993). For example, Conversely, Wayne and Green (1993) found no relationship between high quality LMX relationships and the use of self-focused impression management strategiessuch as self-promotion. This may be due to the fact that supervisors of subordinates in high quality LMX relationships recognize their accomplishments without prompt- ing from the subordinate. On the other hand, ingratiation focusing on ones self may be a useful tool when trying to create, rather than maintain, a high quality exchange between a supervisor and subordinate. This suggests that subordinates in low quality LMX relationships may use self-focused tactics in order to appear worthy of a pro- motion to a high quality relationship. Therefore, we propose Hypothesis 2a. Higher quality leader-member exchange will be positively related to other-focused (other-enhancements, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) in- gratiatory behaviors. Hypothesis 2b. Lower quality leader-member exchange will be positively related to self-focused (self-promotion) ingratiatory behaviors. 5. Dispositional antecedents The importance of dispositional variables to the process of impression manage- ment and more specifically ingratiation, once again can be seen by their inclusion in many models of ingratiation or impression management (Bozeman & Kacmar, 1997; Gilmore et al., 1999). However, only a modest amount of empirical research has been conducted to test this notion (Kristof-Brown et al., 2002). From a disposi- tional perspective, people are generally believed to engage in ingratiation activities not because of the context of the situation but because they possess personality traits that predispose them to engage in such behaviors (Jones & Pittman, 1982; Liden & Mitchell, 1988). In the present study, using role theory as a guide, we examine four dispositional factors, self-esteem, need for power, job involvement, and shyness, that are believed to be related to using other-focused ingratiation tactics to fulfill onesWayne and Green (1993) found that subordinates in a high quality LMX relation- ship who already receive special treatment from the supervisor, still engage in other-focused impression management behaviors such as other-enhancement, opin- ion conformity, and favor rendering. Ingratiation attempts by subordinates who al- ready enjoy a high quality LMX relationship are most likely used to maintain or solidify the quality of their relationship. On the other hand, subordinates in low quality LMX relationships may use ingratiation attempts in the hope of increasing the quality of their relationship with their supervisor. However, ingratiation at- tempts enacted by individuals with a lower status (e.g., subordinates in low quality LMX relationships) are viewed with suspicion limiting their effectiveness (Gurevitch, 1984). Once a subordinate in a low quality LMX relationship learns that his or her other-focused ingratiation attempts are not producing the desired effects, s/he will most likely cease using them.need to be liked or self-focused ingratiation tactics to appear competent. 5.1. Self-esteem Self-esteem is an attitude one holds about the self (Baumeister, Tice, & Hutton, 1989). Individuals with low self-esteem possess a less favorable, but more realistic picture of their attributes than do individuals with high self-esteem (Baumgardner, Kaufman, & Levy, 1989). For example, individuals with low self-esteem are more likely to believe that they will fail when compared to those with high self-esteem (Tice & Baumeister, 1990). However, regardless of ones level of self-esteem, research has shown that people embrace positive and repel negative information about them- selves as a means of enhancing and protecting their self-esteem (Baumgardner et al., 1989; Rosenfeld, 1990). What differs between individuals low and high in self-esteem is how they go about protecting and enhancing their self-esteem, and this can shape role exchange as well as impression management in numerous ways. From an impression management perspective, research has shown that individuals with low self-esteemwill utilizemore self-protective strategieswhile thosewith high lev- els of self-esteem will use more self-enhancing strategies (Baumeister et al., 1989). Ap- plying this rationale to the present study suggests that individuals with low self-esteem will use more other-focused impression management tactics (e.g., other-enhancing, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) than will individuals with high self-esteem because they are self-protective in nature. For example, individuals with low self-es- teem are more likely to agree with anothers opinion rather than stating their own to protect themselves from disagreements with others. On the other hand, self-focused tactics (i.e., self-promotion) are more likely to be used by those high in self-esteem be- cause they offer individuals an opportunity to ‘‘toot ones own horn,’’ something only individuals high in self-esteem would be likely to do. Therefore, we predict Hypothesis 3a. Low self-esteem will be positively related to other-focused (other- enhancements, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) ingratiatory behaviors. Hypothesis 3b. High self-esteem will be positively related to self-focused (self-pro- motion) ingratiatory behaviors. 5.2. Need for power The second dispositional variable considered is the need for power. The need for power is a basic desire to influence and lead others to control a persons own envi- ronment. One way through which influence over others can be accomplished is by creating bases of power (e.g., referent, expert). Power bases can be established by en- acting certain ingratiation tactics (Ralston & Elsass, 1989). For example, self-promo- tion can be used to develop ones expert power base by making others aware of ones knowledge and experience in a particular assigned role. Thus, we expect that individ- uals with a high need for power will engage in self-focused ingratiation tactics in or- der to develop sufficient bases of power that will enable them to maximize control over their environments (Ralston & Elsass, 1989). Conversely, it may be more diffi-cult for high need for power individuals to engage in other-focused tactics as these tactics tend to be associated with subservience (Jones & Wortman, 1973). However, the same cannot be said for low need for power individuals. Individuals who have a low need for power do not care to expand their power bases. Therefore, these indi- viduals should be more willing to use other-focused ingratiation tactics because they are not driven by the need for power themselves and will allow the target to be more powerful (Ralston & Elsass, 1989). These individuals may be more disposed to en- gage in subservient, other-enhancements, especially in instances when the underlying goal is to simply be liked by the target (Jones & Pittman, 1982). This is consistent with the central hypothesis of Heiders (1958) balance theory, which states that sub- ordinates will be driven to achieve a balance of sentiment between themselves and their supervisors. Thus, in the work context, subordinates with a low need for power may be motivated to use other-focused behaviors as a means of balancing the affect between them. Based on this rationale, we predict Hypothesis 4a. Low need for power will be positively related to other-focused (other- enhancements, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) ingratiatory behaviors. Hypothesis 4b. High need for power will be positively related to self-focused (self- promotion) ingratiatory behaviors. 5.3. Job involvement The third dispositional variable we considered, job involvement, is the degree to which individuals actively participate in their jobs (Allport, 1943; Blau & Boal, 1987). Involvement here deals with the individuals psychological state in that it fo- cuses much more on the individual incumbent of a job as opposed to a more situa- tional perspective which looks at the characteristics of the job. Once again, role theory can be used to explain the expected relationship between ingratiation and job involvement. Job involvement is a form of role involvement. The stronger the in- dividuals involvement in his/her job, the more important the job and rewards earned on the job become (Keller, 1997; Stewart-Belle & Lust, 1999). High involvement in ones job role will lead an individual to engage in self-focused (self-promotion) behav- iors in order to be viewed as competent in the job in which he or she is so involved and to help ensure that he or she is positioned to receive the rewards associated with a job well done. Individuals with high levels of job involvement will be willing to make greater investment in any job-related activity that they perceive will help them to se- cure valued rewards in their current jobs or gain advancement into a better position. Thus, such individuals are expected to be more willing to engage in other-focused be- haviors, as well. Through the use of other-focused ingratiatory behaviors, individuals may secure work-related recognition and rewards that will most likely not come through job performance alone. Based on this logic, we propose Hypothesis 5. High job involvement will be positively related to other-focused (other-enhancements, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) and self-focused(self-promotion) ingratiatory behaviors. Shyness is discomfort or inhibition in the presence of others (Cheek & Buss, 1981). According to the self-presentational theory of social anxiety (Leary & Schlenker,1981; Schlenker & Leary, 1985), shy individuals experience heightened levels of so- cial anxiety when they are motivated to manage their impressions for other people but doubt they will succeed in their impression management efforts. This theory pos- tulates that shy people are inhibited because they fear making undesirable impres- sions and feel uncomfortable in such situations. Thus, shy individuals will be especially motivated to manage their impressions while at the same time experience great doubt in their ability to do so. Shy individuals struggle to achieve social accep- tance (Leary & Buckley, 2001). It may be reasoned that in this effort the preeminent goal of an anxious individual becomes avoiding disapproval rather than actively seeking social approval (Shepperd & Arkin, 1990). Ingratiation can be used in a va- riety of ways to achieve this goal. First, ingratiation, or more specifically the other- focused tactic of opinion conformity, can be used to minimize the risk of rejection by expressing divergent opinions or attitudes (Santee & Maslach, 1982). Agreeing with others also will increase affect for the ingratiator making it more difficult to disap- prove of him or her (Heider, 1958; Jones & Pittman, 1982). Second, by enacting other-enhancing ingratiation tactics, the conversation is focused on the target and away from the speaker. By keeping the spotlight focused on others, a shy individual can avoid saying or doing anything that might lead to disapproval by others. How- ever, individuals who are not shy may wish to take center stage. To do this they can enact self-focused ingratiation tactics to promote themselves in the presence of oth- ers. Thus, we believe that individuals who are shy will engage in other-focused ingra- tiatory tactics to avoid the disapproval of others while individuals who are not shy will elect to use self-focused tactics to promote themselves. Therefore, we predict Hypothesis 6a. High levels of shyness will be positively related to other-focused (other-enhancements, opinion conformity, and favor rendering) ingratiatory behaviors. Hypothesis 6b. Low levels of shyness will be positively related to self-focused (self- promotion) ingratiatory behaviors. 6. Value added of dispositional factors beyond situation factors The current approach to predicting ingratiation requires both situational and dispositional variables be considered simultaneously. To date, the context of the situation with respect to impression management and ingratiation has received more5.4. Shyness The final dispositional factor we will consider is that of shyness. While shyness has not been previously considered in the ingratiation literature we believe it is a variable that could play a significant role in determining whether such behaviors are enacted.research attention than dispositional factors and is commonly believed to be the more a return envelope pre-addressed to the researchers, were distributed to individuals employed by the lottery located throughout the state via the agencys inter-organiza- tional mail service. This survey was the employee opinion component of the annual organizational assessment. The directors letter stressed accuracy and timeliness in completing and returning the surveys and assured anonymity. In an effort to solicit as many responses as possible, email reminders were sent to all employees by the agency director 2 and 4 weeks after the surveys were mailed. 7.3. Measures All anchors for all of the scales were strongly disagree on the low end (1) and strongly agree on the high end (5). The reverse coded items were recoded such that high values represented high levels of the constructs. 7.3.1. Ingratiation The scale we used to measure ingratiation was the Measure of Ingratiatory Behav-important of the two (e.g., Eder & Buckley, 1988; Ferris & Judge, 1991; Gilmore et al., 1999). However, in Liden andMitchells (1988) model of ingratiatory behaviors, both antecedents to ingratiation—internal (i.e., disposition) and external characteristics (i.e., situation)—were deemed to be important predictors. Situational variables are un- der the control of the target but dispositional variables are a part of the actor and by considering both a more complete picture is given. We believe that including both fac- tors in onemodel is an important next step in ingratiation research and predict that the dispositional variables will explain additional variance beyond the situational factors. Thus, including the dispositional variables will explain a unique component of ingra- tiatory behaviors that cannot be captured by only considering the situation. Hence, Hypothesis 7. Dispositional factors will explain additional significant variance be- yond situational factors as antecedents to other-focused and self-focused ingratia- tory behaviors. 7. Method 7.1. Sample A total of 136 (response rate of 35%) state lottery employees responded to a sur- vey distributed on company time. Of those who provided demographic information, 54% were males and 46% were females, 72% were Caucasian, the average age was 43 years, and average organizational tenure was 6.77 years. 7.2. Procedure The surveys, which included a letter written by the director of the state lottery andiors in Organizational Settings (MIBOS) developed by Kumar and Beyerlein (1991). It consists of 24 items that assess the 4 dimensions of ingratiation: other-enhance- ment (a ¼ :82), favor rendering (a ¼ :81), opinion conformity (a ¼ :79), and self-pro- motion (a ¼ :71). In its developmental phases, this scale was tested on samples of working students as well as manufacturing employees to provide evidence in support of convergent and discriminant validity (Kumar & Beyerlein, 1991). Sample items from each dimension include: ‘‘Tell him/her that you can learn a lot from his/her ex- perience,’’ ‘‘Go out of your way to run an errand for your supervisor.’’ ‘‘Let your supervisor know the attitudes you share with him/her,’’ and ‘‘Try to make sure he/ she is aware of your successes’’ respectively. To confirm the factor structure of the scale, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis using LISREL 8.3. The model in- cluded four latent variables, one for each ingratiation dimension, composed of the relevant items. Results (X 2 ¼ 278:22, p ¼ 0:0, X 2=df ¼ 1:48, GFI¼ .85, CFI ¼ .90, and RMSEA¼ .06) illustrated adequate fit for the 4-factor model. 7.3.2. Role ambiguity Role ambiguity was measured using five items from the Rizzo, House, and Lirtz- mans (1970) scale. ‘‘I feel certain about how much authority I have at work’’ is a sample item. The internal consistency for this scale was .70. 7.3.3. Leader-member exchange The 7-item leader-member exchange (LMX) scale from the subordinates perspec- tive was used to measure perceptions of the quality of the supervisor–subordinate re- lationship (Scandura et al., 1986). A sample item from this scale is ‘‘My supervisor understands my problems and needs.’’ The internal reliability estimate was .89. 7.3.4. Self-esteem The 9-item self-esteem scale (a ¼ :73) employed in the present study was devel- oped by Rosenberg (1965). A sample item is ‘‘I feel that I have a number of good qualities.’’ 7.3.5. Need for power Need for power was measured using 5-items from Steers and Braunsteins (1976) Manifest Needs Questionnaire. A sample item is ‘‘I strive to gain more control over the events around me at work.’’ The internal consistency of these items was .71. 7.3.6. Job involvement Three items were used to measure job involvement. Two of the items were from Buchanans (1964) involvement scale and one item was from Lobel and Clair (1992). A sample item is ‘‘I am very much personally involved in my work.’’ The items combined to produce an internal reliability of .71. 7.3.7. Shyness The 6-item shyness scale used in the present study was developed by Scheier and Carver (1985). A sample item is ‘‘It takes me time to overcome my shyness in newsituations.’’ The internal reliability estimate for this scale was .75. 8. Results The means, standard deviations, and correlations for the variables of interest can be found in Table 1. As expected, the four ingratiation tactics were very highly cor- related with one another. While job involvement was significantly correlated with the four ingratiation tactics, none of the other antecedents illustrated a consistent pat- tern of intercorrelations with either the antecedents or the outcome variables. 8.1. Common method variance As with all self-report data, there is the potential for the occurrence of method variance. To test for the extent of method variance in the current data we imple- mented the procedure recommended by Widaman (1985) and used by Williams, Cote, and Buckley (1989). Following this approach, four models are estimated, a null measurement model, a model with a single method factor, a multi-factor ‘‘trait’’ measurement model, and a trait measurement model with an additional method fac- tor. If a method factor exists, a model with a method factor will fit the data signif- icantly better than a model with no method factor. In addition, this analysis allows for the partitioning of variance accounted for by different factors and unique sources. More specifically, the sum of the squared loadings can be used to index the total amount of variation due to trait factors, method factors, and unique vari- ance. Results from these analyses indicated that while the method factor slightly im- proved model fit, it accounted for an extremely small portion (6%) of the total var- iance, much less than the amount of method variance (25%) observed by Williams et al. (1989). The results of these analyses suggest that the model tested does benefit from the addition of a method factor. However, the gain in fit is quite small and more importantly the method factor appears to account for very little variation in the data. Therefore, the results suggest that common method variance is not a per- vasive problem in this study and that the relationships observed represent substan- tive rather than artifactual effects. 8.2. Test of the hypotheses Four separate hierarchical regression equations were run to test the relationships of interest in this study, one for each of the four ingratiation dimensions (i.e., other- enhancement, favor rendering, opinion conformity, and self-promotion) as the de- pendent variable. In step one, the control variables of gender and organizational ten- ure were entered. These variables were included as controls given that previous research has suggested that relationships exist between them and ingratiatory behav- iors (e.g., Gordon, 1996; Wayne, Shore, & Liden, 1997). In the second step, the sit- uational variables of role ambiguity and leader-member exchange were entered as a block. The dispositional variables of self-esteem, need for power, job involvement, and shyness were entered on the third step, also as a block. Based on the directionalrelationships hypothesized, one-tailed tests were conducted. Table 1 Means, standard deviations, and correlations Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 Role ambiguity 2.19 .70 — 2 Leader-member exchange 3.63 .89 ).25 — 3 Shyness 2.44 .80 .08 .09 — 4 Self-esteem 4.58 .45 ).21 .02 ).19 — 5 Need for power 3.63 .75 ).15 .06 ).38 .13 — 6 Job involvement 2.86 .82 ).02 .15 ).09 ).10 .24 — 7 Other-enhancing 2.33 .78 .17 .25 .10 ).30 ).18 .34 — 8 Opinion conformity 2.34 .74 .17 ).09 .15 ).32 ).09 .24 .73 — 9 Favor rendering 2.22 .83 .15 .15 .18 ).36 ).16 .21 .73 .69 — 10 Self-promotion 2.50 .87 .15 ).06 .12 ).23 .08 .22 .44 .54 .46 — Note. N ¼ 136. Scale anchors (1, strongly disagree; 5, strongly agree). * p < :05. For comparison purposes, the results for all four regression equations are shown in Table 2. The first outcome examined was other-enhancing. The control variables did not yield a significant amount of explained variance indicating that they were not predictive of other-enhancing behaviors. The change in R2 for the situational vari- ables was significant. The situational antecedent, role ambiguity (H1a) was not sig- nificantly related to other-enhancing behaviors, but LMX (H2a) did explain a significant amount of variance in the predicted direction. Results indicated that the combined effect of the dispositional variables also was significant with self-esteem (H3a), need for power (H4a), and job involvement (H5) explaining significant addi- tional variance in the use of other-enhancing behavior in the expected direction. No support was found for H6a as there was no significant relationship between shyness and other-enhancing behaviors. Finally, as predicted, the dispositional variables ex- plained a significant additional 14% of variance in other-enhancing behaviors (H7). The second regression equation used the ingratiation tactic of opinion conformity as the dependent variable. Here again, the control variables did not prove to be significant. Also, there was no significant relationship found between opinion con- formity and either of the situational variables, role ambiguity (H1a) or leader-mem- ber exchange (H2a). However, the dispositional variables did yield significant relationships. As can be seen in column 2 of Table 2, self-esteem (H3a) and job in- volvement (H5) were significant and in the hypothesized directions, but need for power (H4a) and shyness (H6a) were not. Supporting H7, the dispositional variables explained an additional 10% of the variance over and above the variance explained by the situational and control variables. Table 2 Impact of antecedents on ingratiatory behaviors Other- enhancing Opinion conformity Favor rendering Self- promotion b DR2 b DR2 b DR2 b DR2 Control variables: Gender .006 .062 .043 .141 Organizational tenure ).121 .017 ).076 .021 ).085 .028 ).137 .029 Situational variables: Role ambiguity .109 .114 .126 .172a Leader-member exchange .161a .072a ).089 .051 .137 .067a ).022 .047 Dispositional variables: Self-esteem ).190a ).203a ).238b ).107 Need for power ).220a ).065 ).186a .187a Job involvement .389c .306c .219b .279b Shyness .016 .208c .130 .148c .076 .144c .222a .136b Note. Standardized coefficients shown. R2 change significance shown at each step. a p < :05. b p < :01. c p < :001. 9. DiscussionThe findings of this study support the combined person-situation perspective that argues the need to consider both situational and dispositional variables when exam- ining antecedents of ingratiatory behaviors. For each of the four behaviors examined the dispositional variables explained variance above and beyond the variance ex- plained by the situational factors. In addition, the antecedents that explained each of the ingratiatory behaviors were unique. This suggests that the antecedents that would influence someone to engage in ingratiatory behavior differ by tactic and fu- ture research should study the ingratiation process in this light. One contribution offered by this study was the introduction of several variables that had not previously been tested as antecedents of ingratiatory behaviors. Inclu- sion of each of these variables was supported by theory. One such dispositional var- iable was job involvement. While this variable has not been previously considered in the ingratiation research, it appears to play an important role in the engagement of ingratiation, as it was a significant predictor of all four ingratiatory behaviors stud- ied. As role theory predicted and our results supported, individuals who are involved in their job may be more inclined to employ ingratiation than those less involved in their jobs. Another unique contribution is the fact that shyness had not been consid- ered previously as a dispositional antecedent to ingratiation. Our results also shed light on areas where previous research findings were not clear or consistent. For example, the relationship between self-esteem and ingratia-Next, favor rendering was considered as the dependent variable. As with the pre- viously considered other-focused tactics, there was no effect for the control variables. Although, the combined effect of the situational variables was significant, role ambi- guity (H1a) and leader-member exchange (H2a) were not. The dispositional vari- ables did yield a significant effect with self-esteem (H3a), need for power (H4a), and job involvement (H5) each explaining significant variance in the predicted direc- tion. However, shyness (H6a) was not significant. Finally, the dispositional variables explained an additional 8% of variance beyond the situational and control variables (H7). The final ingratiatory behavior examined was the self-focused behavior of self- promotion. Once again, the control variables did not produce a significant model. Although the combined effect for situational variables was not significant, role am- biguity (H1b) did explain a significant amount of variance in the use of self-promo- tion in the predicted direction. However, leader-member exchange (H2b) was not significant. The dispositional variables of need for power (H4b), job involvement (H5), and shyness (H6b) played a significant role in explaining variance in self-pro- motion. However, high levels, rather than low levels, of shyness, were related to self- promotion, which is opposite of our original prediction. Further, H3b (self-esteem) was not supported. Finally, the dispositional variables explained an additional 9% of variance in self-promotion beyond the situational and control variables (H7).tion has not been well established in the literature (Baumeister et al., 1989). The pres- ent studys findings suggest a strong, negative relationship between self-esteem and other-focused ingratiation. This finding is consistent with our predictions that low self-esteem individuals will use other-focused tactics because they are self-protective in nature. However, the expected relationship between high self-esteem and self-fo- cused behaviors was not found which suggests that previous research may have been confounding the ingratiatory behaviors. That is, lumping both other- and self-fo- cused impression management tactics into one measure may have masked the true relationships among these variables. All of these findings suggest the need to employ a more comprehensive, but refined approach when studying the antecedents of ingra- tiatory behaviors. One of the most interesting findings of this study is that while each of the six an- tecedents considered in this study were significantly related to at least one ingratia- tory behavior they did not operate the same across all four behaviors. Of the situational variables considered, role ambiguity was only significantly related to the self-focused behavior of self-promotion. One interpretation of this finding is that individuals may realize that self-promotion tactics can be used to create the attribu- tion of competence in an ambiguous environment. For example, highlighting ones organizational skills in an unorganized environment may produce an image of com- petence. The situational variable of leader-member exchange was only significant for the other-focused behavior of other-enhancing. As predicted, this relationship was posi- tive indicating that individuals who already share a positive relationship with their supervisor are more likely to engage in other-enhancing behaviors than those who enjoy a less favorable relationship with their supervisor (Wayne & Green, 1993). This finding is consistent with the ingratiators dilemma (Jones & Pittman, 1982; Ralston & Elsass, 1989). That is, when an ingratiators status is lower than the targets, the motivation to ingratiate increases, but the probability that the ingratiation tactics will work decreases. Thus, individuals who do not enjoy a high quality LMX rela- tionship with their supervisor most likely recognize that ingratiation attempts will not be successful and refrain from using them. Similarly, the dispositional variables employed in this study had unique relation- ships with the ingratiatory behaviors. The variable of self-esteem was significantly and negatively related to all of the ingratiatory behaviors except self-promotion. The consistently negative relationships signify that low self-esteem is driving the results. Specifically, individuals with low self-esteem use all of the other-focused in- gratiatory tactics, but not the self-focused behavior. That is, individuals with low self-esteem may have a difficult time bragging about themselves, but can easily fall into the subservient role and position others as the center of attention. The signifi- cant relationships between need for power and the other-focused ingratiatory behav- iors of other-enhancing and favor rendering suggest that individuals with a high need for power may be less inclined to use other-focused tactics. It may be that recogniz- ing important qualities in others and conforming to their opinions are difficult be- haviors for an individual with a high need for power to enact. The dispositional variable of shyness predicted only self-promotion, and, interestingly, the relationshipwas positive. Shy individuals do not wish to make an undesirable impression and ac- tively seek to avoid disapproval (Leary & Buckley, 2001). Thus, shy individuals may use self-promotion tactics to draw attention to their work behaviors in an effort to produce positive reactions from others. Finally, the dispositional variable of job involvement was found to be an impor- tant predictor of all four types of ingratiatory behaviors, both other- and self-fo- cused. In each case, the more involved an individual was in his or her particular job the more likely he or she was to use any form of ingratiation available. These findings offer some confirmation of our contention that the reward power inherent in the supervisors role makes him or her a target of all types of ingratiatory behav- iors designed to secure rewards valued by job involved individuals (Jones & Pittman, 1982; Ralston & Elsass, 1989). In addition to looking at each antecedent individually, this study explored whether dispositional variables could explain additional variance above and beyond that explained by traditional situational variables by employing a combined disposi- tional–situational perspective. Confirming our prediction, in each of the four ingra- tiatory behaviors considered in this study, the dispositional variables explained a significant 8–14% of the variance beyond that explained by the situation. These find- ings beg for future researchers to consider both dispositional and situational vari- ables when studying the process of ingratiation. However, the unique results for each tactic suggest that care should be used when selecting antecedents for future in- gratiation studies as not all antecedents worked the same and should not be consid- ered interchangeable. As with all empirical research there are limitations with the current study. One limitation is that the lack of longitudinal data prevented an assessment of causality. Given that acquiring the attributions sought when using ingratiation tactics may take time, studying this phenomenon longitudinally may be a more accurate ap- proach. A second limitation is the sample included people from only one organiza- tion, perhaps limiting the generalizability of our findings. Only replications with other samples will be able to determine the extent of this limitation. Another limita- tion is that only six antecedents were considered in this study. While there was a strong theoretical rationale for including these variables, these are only a small sam- ple of the possible factors that could influence ingratiatory behaviors. While the cur- rent study was a much needed beginning into this inquiry, future research should consider other dispositional variables such as the big five personality variables and situational variables like organizational design and types of personnel policies when examining the process of ingratiation (Barrick & Mount, 1993; Kristof-Brown et al., 2002; Liden & Mitchell, 1988). Another interesting topic for future research is to ex- amine the impact of supervisor gender and tenure with supervisor on the use of in- gratiation by subordinates. Although past research has examined the impact of subordinate gender on this process (Ferris, Frink, Bhawuk, Zhou, & Gilmore, 1996; Singh, Kumra, & Vinnicombe, 2002) and the demographic similarity of super- visors and subordinates (Wayne & Liden, 1995), supervisor gender and tenure with supervisor have not been examined to date and may have interesting effects on the ingratiation process. For example, some ingratiation tactics may be more appropri-ate for men than for women or the length of tenure with supervisor may impact the ies, have combined these dimensions therefore confounding the unique relationships antecedents have with different ingratiatory behaviors.The goals of ingratiation, to appear competent to the supervisor or to be liked by the supervisor (Jones & Pittman, 1982), are important avenues for career develop- ment. These goals can be accomplished by using either other-focused or self-focused ingratiation tactics (Jones & Wortman, 1973). Our results indicate that, individuals with low self esteem, low need for power, those involved in their jobs, or who have positive relations with their supervisor can effectively engage in other-focused ingra- tiatory behaviors. Furthermore, individuals in ambiguous situations, who are shy, or have a high need for power, can effectively engage in self-focus ingratiatory behav- iors. As existing research suggests, the career benefits for engaging in ingratiatory be- haviors include positive performance appraisals (Wayne & Kacmar, 1991), premier job assignments (Liden & Mitchell, 1988), promotions, and raises (Wayne & Liden, 1995). Over the past forty years a great deal has been learned about the process of ingra- tiation. Little by little each study has added to the foundation of knowledge in this area. We believe that our study illustrates the importance of thinking comprehen- sively, but specifically, about the ingratiation process. From a comprehensive per- spective, researchers would be well served to study global models of the process of ingratiation. However, researchers need to be extremely specific when selecting the variables of interest to ensure that a blanket approach, one that assumes that all vari- ables impact the ingratiation process the same, is not used. References Allport, G. (1943). The ego in contemporary psychology. Psychological Review, 50, 451–478. Anderson, N., Silverster, J., Cunningham-Snell, N., & Haddleton, E. (1999). Relationships between candidate self-monitoring, perceived personality, and selection interview outcomes. Human Relations, 52, 1115–1131. Appelbaum, S. H., & Hughes, B. (1998). Ingratiation as a political tactic: Effects within the organization. Management Decision, 36, 85–95. Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. R. (1993). Autonomy as a moderator of the relationships between the big five personality dimensions and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 111–118. Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Hutton, D. G. (1989). Self-presentational motivations and personalityselection and effectiveness of the ingratiatory behaviors. Finally, future research could benefit from using other measures of impression management (e.g., Bolino & Turnley, 1999) to gain further insights into both situational and dispositional an- tecedents. Despite the limitations, this paper also has several strengths. One strength is that it is one of few empirical studies to examine multiple antecedents of ingratiatory be- haviors in a single study. In addition, using an additive model allowed us to include both situational and dispositional variables, something that has not previously been examined in relation to ingratiatory behaviors. Finally, the examination of both self and other-focused ingratiatory behaviors in one study is a strength. Most past stud-differences in self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 57, 547–575. Baumgardner, A. H., Kaufman, C. M., & Levy, P. E. (1989). Regulating affect interpersonally: When low esteem leads to greater enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 907–921. Blau, G. J., & Boal, K. B. (1987). Conceptualizing how job involvement and organizational commitment affect turnover and absenteeism. Academy of Management Review, 12, 288–300. Bolino, M. C., & Turnley, W. H. (1999). Measuring impression management in organizations: A scale development based on the Jones and Pittman taxonomy. Organizational Research Methods, 2, 187–206. Bolino, M. C., & Turnley, W. H. (2003). More than one way to make an impression: Exploring profiles of impression management. Journal of Management, 29, 141–160. Borman, W. C., White, L. A., & Hedge, J. W. (1997). Personnel selection. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 299–337. Bozeman, D. P., & Kacmar, K. M. (1997). A cybernetic model of impression management processes in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 9–30. Buchanan, B. (1964). Building organizational commitment: The socialization of managers in work organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 19, 533–546. Chatman, J. A. (1989). Improving interactional organizational research: A model of person-organization fit. Academy of Management Review, 14, 333–349. Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 330–339. Day, D. V., & Crain, E. C. (1992). The role of affect and ability in initial exchange quality perceptions. Group and Organization Management, 17, 380–397. Delery, J. E., & Kacmar, K. M. (1998). The influence of applicant and interviewer characteristics on the use of impression management. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1649–1669. Deluga, R. J., & Perry, J. T. (1991). The relationship of subordinate upward influencing behavior, satisfaction, and perceived superior effectiveness with leader-member exchanges. Journal of Occupa- tional Psychology, 64, 239–252. Deluga, R. J., & Perry, J. T. (1994). The role of subordinate performance and ingratiation in leader- member exchanges. Group and Organization Management, 19, 67–86. Dienesch, R. M., & Liden, R. C. (1986). Leader-member exchange model of leadership: A critique and further development. Academy of Management Review, 11, 618–634. Dockery, T. M., & Steiner, D. D. (1990). The role of the initial interaction in leader-member exchange. Group and Organization Studies, 15, 395–413. Duchon, D., Green, S. G., & Tabor, T. D. (1986). Vertical dyad linkage: A longitudinal assessment of antecedents, measures, and consequences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 56–60. Eder, R. W., & Buckley, M. R. (1988). The employment interview: An interactionist perspective. In G. R. Ferris & K. M. Rowland (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 6, pp. 75–107). Greenwich, CT: JAI. Ellis, A. P. J.,West, B. J., Ryan,A.M., &DeShon, R. P. (2002). The use of impressionmanagement tactics in structured interviews: A function of question type? Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 1200–1208. Ferris, G. R., Frink, D. D., Bhawuk, D. P. S., Zhou, J., & Gilmore, D. C. (1996). Reactions of diverse groups to politics in the workplace. Journal of Management, 44, 591–605. Ferris, G. R., & Judge, T. A. (1991). Personnel/human resources management: A political influence perspective. Journal of Management, 17, 447–488. Fletcher, C. (1990). The relationships between candidate personality, self-presentation strategies, and interviewer assessments in selection interviews: An empirical study. Human Relations, 43, 739–749. Gilmore, D. C., Stevens, C. K., Harrell-Cook, G., & Ferris, G. R. (1999). Impression management tactics. In R. W. Eder & M. M. Harris (Eds.), The employment interview handbook. Sage Publications. Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Gordon, R. A. (1996). Impact of ingratiation on judgments and evaluations: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 54–70. Graen, G. B. (1976). Role-making processes within complex organizations. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1201–1245). Chicago: Rand McNally. Graen, G., & Scandura, T. A. (1987). Toward a psychology of dyadic organizing. In L. L. Cummings & B. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 175–208). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Gurevitch, Z. D. (1984). Impression-formation during tactical self-presentation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47(3), 262–270. Hattrup, K., & Jackson, S. E. (1996). Learning about individual differences by taking situations seriously. In K. R. Murphy (Ed.), Individual differences and behavior in organization (pp. 507–547). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Jones, E. E. (1964). Ingratiation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 231–262). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Jones, E. E., & Wortman, C. B. (1973). Ingratiation: An attributional approach. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Kacmar, K. M., Delery, J. E., & Ferris, G. R. (1992). Differential effectiveness of applicant impression management tactics on employment interview decisions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1250–1272. Kahn, R. L., Wolfe, D. M., Quinn, R. P., Snoek, J. D., & Rosenthal, R. A. (1964). Organizational stress. New York: Wiley. Keller, R. T. (1997). Job involvement and organizational commitment as longitudinal predictors of job performance: A study of scientists and engineers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 539– 545. Kinicki, A. J., & Vecchio, R. P. (1994). Influences on the quality of supervisor-subordinate relations: The role of time-pressure, organizational commitment, and locus of control. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 75–82. Kristof-Brown, A., Barrick, M. R., & Franke, M. (2002). Influences and outcomes of applicant impression management use in job interviews. Journal of Management, 28, 27–46. Kumar, K., & Beyerlein, M. (1991). Construction and validation of an instrument for measuring ingratiatory behaviors in organizational settings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 619–627. Leary, M. R., & Buckley, K. E. (2001). Shyness and the pursuit of social acceptance. In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, consolidation, and change (pp. 139–153). New York, NY: Routledge. Leary, M. R., & Schlenker, B. R. (1981). The social psychology of shyness: A self-presentational model. In J. T. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression management theory and social psychological research (pp. 335–358). New York: Academic Press. Liden, R. C., & Mitchell, T. R. (1988). Ingratiatory behaviors in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 13, 572–587. Liden, R. C., Sparrowe, R. T., & Wayne, S. J. (1997). Leader-members exchange theory: The past and potential for the future. In G. R. Ferris (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 15, pp. 47–119). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Lobel, S. A., & Clair, L. (1992). Effects of family responsibilities, gender, and career identity salience on performance outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 55, 1057–1069. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. Newton, T., & Keenan, T. (1991). Further analyses of the dispositional argument in organizational behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 781–787. Phillips, A. S., & Bedeian, A. G. (1994). Leader-follower exchange quality: The role of personal and interpersonal attributes. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 990–1001. Ralston, D. A. (1985). Employee ingratiation: The role of management. Academy of Management Review, 10, 477–487. Ralston, D. A., & Elsass, P. M. (1989). Ingratiation and impression management in the organization. In R. A. Giacalone & P. Roselfeld (Eds.), Impression management in the organization (pp. 235–250). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rizzo, J. R., House, R. J., & Lirtzman, S. E. (1970). Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 15, 150–163. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self image. New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Rosenfeld, P. (1990). Self-esteem and impression management explanations for self-serving bias. The Journal of Social Psychology, 130, 495–500. Rosenfeld, P. R., Giacalone, R. A., & Riordan, C. A. (1995). Impression management in organizations: Theory, measurement, and practice. New York: Routledge. Santee, R. T., & Maslach, G. (1982). To agree or not to agree: Personal dissent amid social pressure to conform. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 690–700. Scandura, T. A., Graen, G. B., & Novak, M. A. (1986). When managers decide not to decide autocratically: An investigation of leader-member exchange and decision influence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 579–584. Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1985). Social anxiety and communication about the self. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, 171–192. Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437–453. Shepperd, J. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1990). Shyness and self-presentation. In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives from social psychology (pp. 286–314). New York: Cambridge University Press. Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1985). Stability in the midst of change: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 465–490. Scheier, M. R., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The self-consciousness scale—A revised version for use with general populations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15(8), 687–699. Singh, V., Kumra, S., & Vinnicombe, S. (2002). Gender and impression management: Playing the promotion game. Journal of Business Ethics, 37, 77–89. Steers, R. M., & Braunstein, D. N. (1976). A behaviorally based measure of manifest needs in work setting. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 9, 251–266. Stevens, C. K., & Kristof, A. L. (1995). Making the right impression: A field study of applicant impression management during job interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 587–606. Stewart-Belle, S., & Lust, J. A. (1999). Career movement of female employees holding lower-level positions: An analysis of the impact of the Type A behavior pattern. Journal of Business & Psychology, 14, 187–197. Tedeschi, J. T., & Melburg, V. (1984). Impression management and influence in the organization. In S. B. Bararach & E. J. Lawler (Eds.), Research in the sociology of organizations (Vol. 3, pp. 31–58). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Self-esteem, self-handicapping, and self-presentation: The strategy of inadequate practice. Journal of Psychology, 58, 443–463. Turnley, W. H., & Bolino, M. C. (2001). Achieving desired images while avoiding undesired images: Exploring the role of self-monitoring in impression management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 351–360. Wayne, S. J., & Ferris, G. R. (1990). Influence tactics, affect, and exchange quality in supervisor- subordinate interactions: A laboratory experiment and a field study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 487–499. Wayne, S. J., & Green, S. A. (1993). The effects of leader-member exchange on employee citizenship and impression management behavior. Human Relations, 46, 1431–1440. Wayne, S. J., & Kacmar, K. M. (1991). The effects of impression management on the performance appraisal process. Organizational Behavior and Human Decisions Processes, 48, 70–88. Wayne, S. J., & Liden, R. C. (1995). Effects of impression management on performance rating: A longitudinal study. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 232–260. Wayne, S. J., Shore, L., & Liden, R. C. (1997). Perceived organizational support and leader-member exchange: A social exchange perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 82–111. Widaman, K. (1985). Hierarchical nested covariance structure models for multitrait-multimethod data. Applied Psychological Measurement, 9, 1–26. Williams, L. J., Cote, J. A., & Buckley, M. R. (1989). Lack of method variance in self-reported affect and perceptions of work: Reality or artifact? Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 462–468. Wortman, C. B., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (1977). Interpersonal attraction and techniques of ingratiation in organizational settings. In B. M. Staw & G. R. Salancik (Eds.), New directions in organizational behavior (pp. 133–178). Chicago: St. Clair Press.