Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience journa l homepage: ht tp : / /www.e lsev ier .com/ locate /dcn An ERP study of conflict monitoring in 4–8-ye Associa Kristin A ra, L a Department o b Department o es a r t i c l Article history: Received 30 A Received in re 13 December 2010 Accepted 29 December 2010 Keywords: ERP N2 Conflict monit Temperament Development t in ide ology, t he N2, linked cognitive and affective risk factors for anxiety. Most of this research, however, has been conducted with adults, adolescents, and older children, but not with younger chil- dren. To address this gap, the current study examined 26 4–8-year-olds, who completed a cued flanker task while EEG was continuously recorded. We assessed whether the N2 was detectable in this group of young children and examined associations between the N2 and 1. Introdu There ha understand cesses (e.g., on theoretic to the deve and adapti 1995; Posne Although th types of cog ical marker ∗ Correspon Building I, The United States. E-mail add 1878-9293/$ – doi:10.1016/j.oring factors reflecting affective risk (e.g., reduced executive attention, temperamental effortful control, and temperamental surgency).We documented anN2 effect (greater N2 amplitude to incongruent versus congruent flankers), but only in children older than 6 years of age. Increases in the N2 effect were associated with less efficient executive attention and lower temperamental effortful control.Wediscuss the implications of these findings and consider how they may inform future studies on biomarkers for cognitive and affective risk factors for anxiety. © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. ction s been a surge of research in recent years on ing the development of cognitive control pro- inhibitoryandattentional control) based inpart al andempirical studies linking theseprocesses lopment of emotion regulation, self-regulation ve behavioral outcomes (e.g., Rothbart et al., r andRothbart, 2007; Lewis andStieben, 2004). ere have been numerous studies linking these nitive control processes with neurophysiolog- s via scalp recorded event-related potentials ding author at: 163 Child Study Center, University Support Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, Tel.: +1 814 863 1715; fax: +1 814 865 7002. ress: kbuss@psu.edu (K.A. Buss). (ERPs), most of this work has been with older children, adolescents and adults. For example, the N2 is a early frontal negativity that is elicited during conflict and inhi- bition tasks (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003) and is believed to be a marker for cognitive control processes, most notably conflict monitoring and detection (Van Veen and Carter, 2002a,b). However, we do not know much about the pres- ence or function of the N2 and the association between the N2 and behavioral indicators of risk and resilience in very young children. Thus, the primary goal of current study was to test whether the N2 (1) is present and (2) varies predictably with degree of stimulus conflict in a sample of typically developing children as young as 4 years of age. Attentional processes, such as executive control, are believed to be important links between early temperament (e.g., fearful behavior) and either adaptive or maladap- tive outcomes (Olvet and Hajcak, 2008; Lewis et al., 2008). For instance, vigilant attention to threat and effortful con- see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. dcn.2010.12.003tions with temperament . Bussa,∗, Tracy A. Dennisb, Rebecca J. Brooke f Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, United States f Psychology, Hunter College of The City University of New York, United Stat e i n f o pril 2010 vised form a b s t r a c t Although there is great interes that create risk for psychopath event-related potential (ERP), tar old children: auren M. Sippela ntifying the neural correlates of cognitive processes here is a paucity of research in young children. One is thought to index conflict monitoring and has been 132 K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 trol have been examined as mechanisms in the etiology of anxiety (Mathews and MacLeod, 2005; Lonigan et al., 2004; Lonigan and Vasey, 2009). Numerous studies have documented physiological differences associated with the developme as fearful/i review) and Rothbart, 2 the temper sis which g behavior (K affective, at play a role i problems (D Perez-Edga tial to serve diathesis as 2009; Denn secondary g the N2 is as have been l tion, loweff surgency. 1.1. N2 and The N2 i frontocentr and 400ms are though trol resourc incorrect re Folstein an those cond reflected in Veen andCa erated duri response te tasks that r (e.g., Go-No (e.g., incong 2008; Kopp and Carter, is expected compared t The N2, (e.g., error- of the ante Carter et al. key region o cessing of b et al., 2004 bulk of this very little r the N2 in ch 1.2. Develo As early which ther Nugent, 19 N2 tends to be larger in children compared to adults and generally decreases with age (Henderson, 2010; Johnstone et al., 2005; Jonkman, 2006; Lewis et al., 2006b; Lewis and Stieben, 2004), although some exceptions have been mente itude i ognitiv itudes contr ; Gold e N2 ar tures, ) and r ). both rators owev ation t , 2006a ctivati vemen ion to is et al addit 2, ind een th across 2 in c r unde l regul lopme et al. t dev ren (ag (ages nse to ren (ag a chan near c s child unders al effe N2 rela iors revie of the e will r ences also m nd sim itive c g the with a er em r regu iated 12 y was , it apnt of temperamental variation in behavior such nhibited behavior (see Fox et al., 2005 for attentional and effortful control (Posner and 007). Specifically, it has been proposed that amental inhibition includes a biological diathe- ives rise to a pattern of fearful and anxious agan et al., 1992). The N2 has been linked to tentional, and cognitive factors that appear to n the emergence of a range ofmood and anxiety ennis and Chen, 2009; Ladouceur et al., 2010; r and Fox, 2005). Thus, the N2 has the poten- as a neurophysiological marker for a biological sociated with affective risk (Dennis and Chen, is et al., 2009; Luu and Tucker, 2004). Thus, a oal of the current study is to examine whether sociated with temperamental differences that inked to affective risk: reduced executive atten- ortful control, highnegative affectivity, and low conflict monitoring s a negative-going waveform that is maximal in al electrodes appearing sometime between 200 after the onset of a stimulus. N2 amplitudes t to reflect the degree to which cognitive con- es are recruited to resolve conflict and inhibit sponses (Braver et al., 2001; Jones et al., 2002; d Van Petten, 2008). Thus, the N2 is largest in itions that involve the most conflict typically increasederror ratesand/or reaction times (Van rter, 2002a,b).Methodologically, theN2 is gen- ng tasks in which two or more incompatible ndencies are activated at the same time such as equire the inhibition of a pre-potent response Go task) or that include incongruent stimuli ruent visual flankers) (Folstein and Van Petten, et al., 1996;Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003; VanVeen 2002a,b). For instance, in a flanker task the N2 to be larger to the incongruent (conflict trials) o the congruent trials (i.e., N2 effect). along with other medial frontal negativities related negativity), has been linked to activity rior cingulate cortex (ACC; Banich et al., 2001; , 1998;VanVeenandCarter, 2002a,b),which is a f the medial frontal cortex involved in the pro- oth cognitive and affective conflict (e.g., Bishop ; Vuilleumier et al., 2001). Again, however, the work has been conducted in adults so there is esearch clarifying the functional significance of ildren. pment of the N2 as age 4, the N2 is evident in contexts in e is cognitive emotional challenge (Nelson and 90; Todd et al., 2007). The amplitude of the docu ampl on c ampl nitive 1997 in th struc 2005 1999 In gene tex; h activ et al. N2 a invol addit (Lew In the N betw able the N large tiona deve Todd sisten child cents respo child gest nonli acros fully ment 1.3. behav As ment As w differ ior is N2 a cogn durin dren great bette assoc 8 and same Thusd (Ladouceur et al., 2004). This decline in N2 s often accompanied by improved performance e tasks (Lamm et al., 2006) and smaller N2 are observed during tasks of attention and cog- ol (Bachevalier and Mishkin, 1984; Casey et al., man et al., 1971). Thus age-related reductions e thought to reflect the maturation of physical including cortical thickening (O’Donnell et al., eductions in grey matter volume (Giedd et al., children and adults, the N2 is linked to neural in medial frontal areas of the cingulate cor- er, children show considerably more posterior han is seen in adults (Jonkman, 2006; Lewis ,b; Stieben et al., 2007). This pattern of greater on in posterior cortical regions suggests the t of more automatic attentional processing in more deliberative top-down cognitive control ., 2006a,b; Stieben et al., 2007). ion to these general developmental changes in ividual variation in N2 amplitudes and links e N2 and emotional traits are somewhat vari- development. In general, the amplitude of hildren compared to adults is expected to be r conditions that require cognitive or emo- ation, suggesting that systemmay still be under nt (Johnstone et al., 2005; Lewis et al., 2006b; , 2007). However, there are also some incon- elopmental findings. For instance, both young es 4–6; Nelson and Nugent, 1990) and adoles- 13–16; Lewis et al., 2006b) show a larger N2 in emotional information relative to school-aged es 7–12 across studies). These differences sug- ging attunement to emotional information or hanges in emotion-related sensitivity of the N2 hood. Thus, the development of the N2 is not tood andmore research documenting develop- cts are needed. tions with affective and cognitive control wed above, findings concerning the develop- N2 in terms of amplitude are somewhatmixed. eview next, the literature examining individual in the association between the N2 and behav- ixed. Research findings demonstrate that the ilar medial frontal negativities thought to tap ontrol are larger and have a shorter latency experience of negative emotion and in chil- nxiety (Lewis et al., 2008). On the other hand, otional flexibility, thought to be indicative of lation and control of negative emotions, is also with larger N2 amplitudes in children between ears of age (Lewis et al., 2006a), although the not true in younger children (5–7-year olds). pears for older children and adolescents that K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 133 both increased negative affect and more effective regula- tionof negative emotions is linked to greater neural “effort” as reflected in larger N2 amplitudes. Given the ongoing development of prefrontal cortical regions in young chil- dren, great recruitmen mance and otherhand, ral inefficie less efficien ies with adu older childr There h association activity. In as high soo ciated with cognitive co In contrast, (2010)dem high-shy ch ent flanker was not a d shyness sug reflecting re sures such a these neura understand aspects of t Given t emergence 2002; Pere in the pre tions betwe Although r amplitudes (the N2 effe et al., 1996; 2002a,b), ot associated mance (i.e., to congruen larger N2 e which resu executive a case in child In the p between th Attention N is a cued fl performanc crete doma (Posner and us to exam N2 and exe a range of a Taken to are inconclu are associat negative aff highlights t incongruent–congruent) because it reflects conflict mon- itoring (incongruent) relative to a non-conflict baseline (congruent); in contrast, most of the research reported above with children only examines correlations between mplitu idual d oth t erame ous stu l reso ction might fect – t possi nt stu elated tful co d the examin onflict he cur ildhoo hich tive pr e ong ol.Mo any a n com cellen treme y the N evelo ap, in t N2 in ren, as NT, an in tem e teste will itudes fect—l t tria -like p cause ehavi ratory to sho e to w with p ypothe with l , we ex rnal re broad affect the l mar ever, w tively cer N2 amplitudes might indicate the effective t of cognitive resources to support task perfor- be associated with better performance. On the greaterN2amplitudes could also reflect a “neu- ncy” pattern (i.e., increased N2 associated with t behavioral performance) found in some stud- lts (e.g., Dennis and Chen, 2009) and studies of en (Lamm and Lewis, 2010). ave been a handful of studies examining the between temperamental variation and N2 children as young as age 7, characteristics such thability or attention control have been asso- larger N2 amplitudes during different types of ntrol tasks (Perez-Edgar and Fox, 2005, 2007). in a sample of children ages 9–12, Henderson onstrated increased social anxietyoutcomes for ildren with larger N2 amplitudes to incongru- trials. However, in the Henderson study there irect association between N2 amplitudes and gesting that perhaps temperamental shyness, activity, may be distinct from regulatory mea- s those indexedby theN2. Thus, examinationof l markers in young children may be critical for ing the relation between reactive and control emperament. he importance of attentional factors in the of mood and anxiety problems (Fox et al., z-Edgar and Fox, 2005; Vasey et al., 1995), sent study we wished to examine associa- en the N2 and executive attention efficiency. esearch with adults suggests that greater N2 to incongruent compared to congruent trials ct) reflects adaptive conflict monitoring (Kopp Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003; Van Veen and Carter, her research suggests that larger N2 effects are with less efficient executive attention perfor- slower reaction times to incongruent compared t trials; Dennis and Chen, 2009). Therefore, a ffect may reflect less efficient control capacity lts in poor task performance (i.e., less efficient ttention). It is unclear whether this is also the ren. resent study, we will examine associations e N2 and attention performance using the etwork Test (ANT; Fan et al., 2002). The ANT anker task that yields measures of attention e in three anatomically and functionally dis- ins: alerting, orienting, and executive attention Petersen, 1990; Fan et al., 2002). Thiswill allow ine the specificity of associations between the cutive attention, or whether the N2 is linked to ttentional processes. gether, these studies with adults and children sive in terms of whether larger N2 amplitudes ed with greater cognitive control and reduced ect, or vice versa. Notably, research with adults he importance of focusing on the N2 effect (i.e., N2 a indiv ine b temp previ neura distin there N2 ef effor prese are r effor exten only the c 1.4. T Ch N2, w affec ing th contr has m tratio an ex of ex natel and d this g in the child the A ences W there ampl N2 ef gruen adult Be and b explo tive degre ated we h ated Next mate three ative Given neura How negades to conflict-only contexts and behavioral ifferences. In the present study, we will exam- o better tease apart how the N2 relates to ntalpredispositionsand tobetter comparewith dies. That is, if the N2 effect reflects activity of urces to resolve the conflict there should be a between congruent and incongruent trials, but beacost toperformance if thatdistinction– the is too large (reflecting inefficiency or too much bly reflecting that task is too difficult). In the dy, we will examine whether greater N2 effects to disrupted attention performance, reduced ntrol, and increased negative affect. This will existing research with children that typically e theN2within trial-type (and typically during trials). rent study d is a period of extensive cognitive change. The is highly sensitive to changes in cognitive and ocessing,maybeparticularly useful formeasur- oing development of emotional and cognitive reover, theuse of ERPs in researchwith children dvantages, including relative ease of adminis- pared to other neuroimaging techniques, and t temporal resolution that allows measurement ly rapid covert cognitive processes. Unfortu- 2 literature as it pertains to child populations pmental processes is quite sparse. To address his study,we examined age-related differences a sample of 4–8-year-old, typically developing sociations with attention performance during d whether the N2 varied with individual differ- perament. d three hypotheses. First,wehypothesized that be age-related changes in the pattern of N2 such that older children would show a clear argerN2 amplitudes to incongruent versus con- ls during the flanker task, reflecting a more attern. results on theassociationbetween theN2effect or are mixed, the next set of hypotheses was . Even though it is developmentally norma- w a larger N2 effect compared to adult, the hich this distinction is made may be associ- roblems. Thus, given the most recent research, sized that a larger N2 effect would be associ- ess efficient executive attention performance. plored the relation between the N2 effect and port of temperament. Specifically, we focus on dimensions of temperament: surgency, neg- , and effortful control (Rothbart et al., 2001). paucity of research linking temperament to kers, these analyses were largely exploratory. e did hypothesize that the N2 effect would be orrelated with the temperament dimension of 134 K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 effortful control which most closely measures the execu- tive functioning, as well as increased negative affect and reduced surgency. 2. Methods 2.1. Participants Thirty-five children participated in the current study. Participants were recruited through fliers and announce- ments in a community newsletter. To be eligible for participation, children had to be between 4 and 8 years of age, right-handed, free of any known neurological impair- ments, andnot takinganystimulantmedications.Of the full sample, fou one child re could not b Thus, 29 ch children pr average age The sample shead index the sample ties. 2.2. Proced 2.2.1. Labor Upon de ticipation i including a tionnaire to visit. Upon with a neu baseline re tory episod and attenti Families re received a s 2.2.2. Atten Children AttentionN Rueda et al. ogy Softwa was presen back to part the task. Ch the compu 1 As a part o with a strange child was seat tant) entered t engaged the c stranger asked “What other t Following this purpose and f it was time fo completion of hold in their lap or place on a table in front of them, whichever was more comfortable. The experimenter explained the task to eachparticipant using a set of index cards depicting an array of five fish. Par- ticipantsw the middle using the re als, the exp button on response fo When it wa started with A sessio tice trials a began s. For rning d for onditi cue. nted ouble of the e spat e posi 0ms f his, th n unti ms el were n and essing irectio he tar direc surrou Accur A sch . Elect G dat nel de analyz eodes 0Hz. (Fer record -bit a r acqu eferen orrect öfer scree n, EG movem ding highp Hz. C rimentr children refused towear the electrode cap and moved the cap during data collection. One visit e completed due to equipment malfunction. ildren (10 females) completed the visit and 26 ovided complete EEG and behavioral data. The of participants was 68.58 months (SD=15.49). was largely middle-class, with mean Holling- of 48.62 (12.62) ranging from 21 to 66; 80% of was Caucasian with 20% racial/ethnic minori- ure atory visit termining that their child was eligible for par- n the study, parents were mailed a packet consent form and a child temperament ques- be completed and brought to the laboratory arrival to the laboratory, children were fitted ral net used for EEG data collection. Following cording, the children participated in labora- es including a conversation with a stranger1 on task followed by a post baseline collection. ceived $20 for their participation and children mall gift. tion network test individually completed a child version of the etworkTest (Dennis et al., 2009; Fanet al., 2002; , 2004) on a Dell PC using E-Prime 1.1 (Psychol- re Tools, Inc.: Pittsburgh, PA). The experimenter t throughout testing, but did not provide feed- icipants outside of encouragement to complete ildren were seated approximately 10 in. from ter screen and given a response box to either f the laboratory visit, children took part in a conversation r episode designed to elicit fear and wariness. While the ed in front of the computer, a stranger (2nd research assis- he physiology chamber, stood next to the child’s chair, and hild in casual conversation for approximately 2min. The questions (e.g., “What kinds of games do you like to play?” hings do you like to do?”) and waited for child to respond. , the stranger spoke, from a script, to the child about the unctions of the EEG net (approximately 1min), stated that r them to leave, and exited the experimental room. After this task, children completed the ANT task. trial 400m a wa sente cue c or no prese the d tions In th in th of 45 ing t scree 1700 pants scree by pr the d als, t same was tion. trial. Fig. 1 2.2.3. EE chan and cal G of 50 70k was a 16 Cz fo age r and c Jungh were Statio eye excee were at 35 expe yses.ere instructed to pay attention only to the fish in of the array (i.e., the target) and “feed that fish” sponse box. Prior to beginning the practice tri- erimenter asked participants to indicate which the response box corresponded to the correct r the target arrays depicted on the index cards. s clear that participants were ready to begin we a set of practice trials. n of the ANT consisted of a total of 16 prac- nd three experimental blocks of 32 trials. Each with the presentation of a fixation cross for purposes of behavioral scoring, on some trials cue replaced the fixation cross and was pre- 150ms and represented one of four warning ons: a center cue, a double cue, a spatial cue, In the center cue condition, an asterisk was at the same location of the fixation cross. In cue condition, an asterisk appeared at loca- target both above and below the fixation cross. ial cue condition, a single asterisk appeared tion of the upcoming target. A fixation period ollowed the disappearance of the cue. Follow- e target array appeared and remained on the l a response was detected or a maximum of apsed, followed by a ITI of 1000ms. Partici- told that a fish (target) would appear on the that they should “feed the fish in the middle” the button on the response box that matched n that the fish was facing. During congruent tri- get fish was surrounded by fish facing in the tion; during incongruent trials, the target fish nded by fish pointing in the opposite direc- acy and reaction time were recorded for each ematic representation of the task is shown in roencephalograph recordings a were recorded during the ANT using a 128- nse array Geodesic Sensor Net (Tucker, 1993) ed using Net Station software from Electri- ics, Inc. (EGI, Eugene, OR) at a sampling rate All impedances were reduced to less than ree et al., 2001) during data acquisition. EEG ed using a 0.1–100Hz bandpass filter with nalog-to-digital converter and referenced to isition and re-referenced offline to the aver- ce (Bertrand et al., 1985; Tucker et al., 1993) ed for polar average reference effects (PARE; et al., 1999) prior to data analysis. Artifacts ned using automatic detection methods (Net I, Inc.) and visually inspected. Eye blink and ent artifacts (70V threshold) and signals 200V were removed during averaging. Data ass filtered at .10Hz and a lowpass filtered hannels with excessive noise throughout the weremarked as “bad” and excluded from anal- K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 135 Fig. 1. Modific Note: Experim 2.3. Coding 2.3.1. Event The EEG 50ms prior divided ac incongruen preceding s eye movem were exclu ical spline i (Perrin et a interpolate one partici was corrupation of attention network task. ental procedure. (a) The four cue conditions; (b) the four stimuli; and (c) an over and data reduction -related potentials was time-locked to the stimulus, segmented to and 500ms following the stimulus, and cording to type of trial (congruent versus t). Epochs were baseline corrected for 50ms timulus onset. Segments containing eye blinks, ents, or response times of less than 200ms ded. Bad channels were replaced using spher- nterpolation of values of neighboring channels l., 1987). The average number of bad channels d in the final data set was 7.56. EEG data from pant was not used because the EEG data file ted. Two children were excluded from analy- ses because may not ha tions. Upon Thus, we a dren. The tim principle co data. Three of the varia peak betwe at approxim (identified a was defined between 3 tracted fromview of the procedure. reaction time performance data indicated they ve been cooperating or understand the instruc- examination of videotapes this was confirmed. nalyzed the data from the remaining 26 chil- e window for the N2 was identified using a mponents analyses with the grand averaged componentswere identified accounting for 93% nce and included a fast positive to negative en 92 and 176ms; a second positive deflection ately 250ms; and second negative deflection s the N2) at 350ms. For each individual, the N2 as the greatest negative deflection occurring 20 and 380ms (±30ms) post-response sub- the preceding positive peak. This deflection 136 K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 was maximal at Cz. This method was selected in order to capture the full degree of the negative deflection and account for possible individual differences in EEG ampli- tudes prior to theN2 (seeNieuwenhuis et al., 2004). N2was calculated a for the con extreme va next lowest 2.3.2. Atten From p ciency of al calculated correct tria cue and ty three atten lated by su cue was p tion (RT no greater ale culated by cues were was presen scores indic spatial cue ing effects attention e reaction tim incongruen scores indic efficient ex normality. E above and value. 2.3.3. Mate Tempera Behavior Q Rothbart, 2 that assess gency/appr Parents res (1 = extrem child, 3 = sl false of my true of my c internal con .65 to .85. 3. Results Thedesc trodesite (F flanker) an presented the task, co gruent trial incongruen Table 1 Means and standard deviations for N2 amplitudes and performance. Congruent Incongruent Fz −10.71 (5.71) −11.74 (6.79) tion tim rrect ting nting flict N2 am ype order s sites ures A ruent, effect itudes (p’s < .0 2.30, i hole. ext, gi age as plit th ). The 12 chi of res as ma these l Type ) ANO action arison a sig 7.77,p led th ruent) 9.05, p the di r olde nstrat itudes e olde Associa eramen e were was t-repo es in children,we also examined theN2 to the congru- nd incongruent trials. Given thatwe only found theN2 t for older children, andagewas correlatedwith theN2 t (r=−.42, p< .05), we conducted partial correlations olling for age. These correlations are summarized in 2. We found that greater N2 effect was associated higher conflict scores, reflecting less efficient exec- attention. The incongruent N2 was also associatedt three midline sites (Fz, Cz, and Pz) separately gruent and incongruent trials. We truncated lues above 1.5 SD (across participants) to the value. tion performance reaction times erformance on the ANT, measures of effi- erting, orienting, and executive attention were from the reaction time (RT) data from the ls. RT to the flanker targets under different pe conditions measures the effects of these tion networks. Alerting efficiency was calcu- btracting the RT for trials in which the double resented from trials with no cue presenta- cue-RT double cue). Higher scores indicate rting efficiency. Orienting efficiency was cal- subtracting the RT for trials in which spatial presented from trials in which a central cue ted (RT center cue-RT spatial cue). Higher ate greater efficiency in orienting because the provides more information than the alter- of the cue alone. Conflict score (i.e., executive fficiency) was calculated by subtracting the e for congruent trials from reaction time for t trials (RT incongruent–RT congruent). Higher ate greater conflict which is interpreted as less ecutive attention. Scores were examined for xtreme values over 1.5 SD (across participants) below the mean were truncated to the next rnal-reported child temperamental shyness mental shyness was assessed using the Child uestionnaire Short Form (CBQ; Putnam and 006a,b). The CBQ short form contains 94 items three broad domains of temperament: sur- oach, negative affect, and effortful control. pond to statements on a 6-point Likert scale ely untrue of my child, 2 =quite untrue of my ightly untrue of my child, 4 =neither true nor child, 5 = slightly true of my child, 6 = extremely hild). All scales have been shown to have good sistency, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from riptive statistics forN2 amplitudes at each elec- z, Cz, Pz)by trial type (incongruentor congruent d attention performance behavioral data are in Table 1. Children performed quite well on mpleting correctly an average of 41 the con- s (range=27–48) and 39 (range=30–48) of the t trials. Cz Pz Reac % co Aler Orie Con 3.1. trial t In acros meas cong main ampl at Pz 50) = as a w N ined We s years and tern N2 w from (Tria older inter comp found 24) = revea cong 11) = with ted fo demo ampl for th 3.2. temp W at Cz paren studi ent a effec effec contr Table with utive−10.03 (5.72) −10.86 (5.82) −6.70 (5.26) −8.31 (4.05) e 911.69 (148.35) 977.50 (166.82) 83.26 (16.74) 76.08 (14.98) 51.87 (54.03) 17.43 (77.37) 65.81 (68.38) plitude differences across electrode sites and to examine whether N2 amplitudes varied , trial types, and age we conducted a repeated NOVA. This 3 (Site: Fz, Cz, Pz)×2 (Trial Type: incongruent) analysis revealed a significant of Site, F(2, 50) =7.37,p< .01,2p = .23, such that at Fz andCzwere significantly larger than those 5) (Fig. 2). Therewas no effect of Trial Type, F(1, ndicating no significant N2 effect for the sample ven the large age range of children, we exam- an additional factor in an additional ANOVA. e sample at the median age (72 months; 6 re were 14 children younger than 72 months ldren older than 72 months. Given the pat- ults in the original ANOVA showing that the ximal at Fz and Cz, the Pz site was dropped analyses. We conducted a 2 (Site: Fz, Cz) by 2 : congruent, incongruent) by 2 (age: younger, VA analysis. There was a significant trial by age , F(1, 24) =5.53, p< .05, 2p = .19. A set of posthoc s were used to examined this interaction. We nificant Trial Type× age interaction at Cz, F(1, < .01,2p = .24, butnot at Fz. Testing this further at the expected N2 effect (N2 incongruent >N2 was only significant for the older children, F(1, < .01, 2p = .45. This effect is depicted in Fig. 3 fference waves (incongruent–congruent) plot- r and younger children at Fz and Cz. In sum, we ed that the expected difference between theN2 by trial type to theflanker targetwas significant r children only and at electrode site Cz. tions between the N2 and performance and t interested in examining whether the N2 effect associated with attention performance and rted temperament. To be consistentwith other K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 137 Fig. 2. with less e associated w reported te scores: Sur Although la surgency, NN2 waveforms for congruent and incongruent trials. fficient alerting but the N2 difference was not ith alerting, or orienting. Turning to maternal- mperament, we focused on the three factor gency, Effortful Control, and Negative Affect. rger incongruent N2 was associated with more 2 effect was not. Greater N2 effect and larger F N2 to incon Effortful Co Affect and c 4. Discussi The curr a measure related diff between N The main g the N2 wa young child the magnit differences a discussion future deve biomarkers 4.1. Eviden conflict mon Consiste effects in th N2 amplitu all children even in pre N2 amplitu not limited widely dist dren comp adult comp gest a fronig. 3. N2 difference waveforms by age group. gruent trials were both associated with less ntrol. Noassociationswere foundwithNegative ongruent N2. on ent study extends the literature on the N2 as of conflict monitoring by demonstrating age- erences in early childhood and associations 2 and executive attention and temperament. oal of the project was to determine whether s associated with conflict monitoring in very ren. A secondary goal was to explore whether ude of the N2 was associated with individual in attention and behavior. We will now turn to of each of these findings and implications for lopmental ERP research, including discussion of for temperamental variation and affective risk. ce for development of the N2 response and itoring effects nt with predictions, we found age-related e modulation of the N2. First, we found that des on all trials were maximal at Fz and Cz for , suggesting frontalization of the N2 response schoolers. As we reviewed in the introduction, des across a variety of studies including but to conflict N2, were more likely to be more ributed and larger at more parietal sites in chil- ared to adults. Although we did not have an arison, the data from the current study sug- tocentral distribution of the conflict N2 even 138 K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 Table 2 Partial correlations, controlling for age, between N2 at Cz and attention performance and temperament. Cz congruent N2 Cz incongruent N2 Cz incongruent-congruent N2 Conflict score −.03 −.29 −.42a Alerting sco a Orienting sc CBQ surgenc CBQ effortfu CBQ negativ a p< .05. b p< .10. in the youn frontalizati a validated researchers effects (e.g. Second, amplitudes was only ev dence that conflict mo hood (>6 in These age demonstrat component of the ACC agedchildre Lewis et al. Third, w effect was tion. Altho associated w congruent) suggesting So while th and presen reflect reso utive attent the finding greater the executive a is the first association 4.2. N2 as a children Turning current stud gruent trial effortful co which refle et al., 2001 ing summa different ty current fin opmental s N2 amplitu regulatoryp trol) (Perez onent nt stud contro mplitu een tr accou at the he N2 be th ren in tful co al reso skills eeded is th ol pro dies o ren mo thoug gruen s whi y. Surg ive af ) and isk for tifter, ugh n e conti y as rep vior co In fact thus w in par lative to tem t is for ). e do k al fron tempe ty (Ha that Mre .18 .41 ore .04 −.11 y .21 −.36b l control −.25 .43a e affect −.10 −.16 gest children. It is possible that we found the on of the N2 in our sample because we used , developmentally appropriate task as other have suggested would enhance detection of , Hogan et al., 2005; Torpey et al., 2009). the expected modulation of the N2 (larger to incongruent compared to congruent trials) ident for the older children. Thus, we have evi- this N2 effect may be a good biomarker for nitoring efficiency in children in early child- our sample) but not in preschool-aged children. differences are consistent with the literature ing age-related changes across several ERP s thought to reflect the ongoing development and prefrontal cortex in preschool and school- n (e.g.,Henderson,2010; Johnstoneetal., 2005; , 2006b; Lewis and Stieben, 2004). e found that after controlling for age, the N2 associated with less efficient executive atten- ugh the N2 to incongruent trials alone was ith alerting, the N2 effect (incongruent versus was not associated with orienting or alerting specificity of links with executive attention. e incongruency effect on the N2 is normative t in older children, greater N2 effect may also urce depletionwhich is onemechanism in exec- ion interference. This finding is consistent with s of Dennis and Chen (2009) showing that the N2 effect was associated with less efficient ttention. To our knowledge, the current study to demonstrate modulation of the N2 and its with executive attention in children this young. biomarker for temperamental variation in to the second, more exploratory goal of the y,we found that largerN2amplitudes to incon- s and the N2 effect were associated with less ntrol. Note that this effortful control finding, cts behavioral control and regulation (Rothbart ) was consistent with the conflict score find- comp curre els of N2 a betw could be th ine t could child effor neur these are n study contr of stu child Al incon score genc posit 2001 and r and S Altho of th genc beha ness. CBQ, ing, specu sitive like i 2009 W medi with anxie noterized above demonstrating consistency across pes of measures of executive functioning. The dings extend the literature, especially devel- tudies demonstrating an association between de and other, related aspects of control and rocesses (e.g., soothability and attentional con- -Edgar and Fox, 2005, 2007) both of which are lescents wh which is a that Hende between te 9–13-year- and adulth tion betwee.21 −.13 −.03 .41a .08 s of the effortful control construct used in the y. However, in these two other studies low lev- l and soothabilitywere associatedwith smaller des to incongruent trials; and the difference ial types (i.e., N2 effect) was not explored. This nt for the differences in findings, or it could differences in the types of tasks used to exam- accounted for the discrepancy. In addition, it at the younger children (compared to age of other studies) in our sample who have low ntrol and self-regulation need to recruit more urces when resolving conflict specifically. As and the PFC develop, fewer neural resources to complete these tasks. To our knowledge, our e first to examine the N2 effect in relation to cesses and a given the relatively small number n the N2 and behavioral control processes in re empirical work is needed. h not hypothesized, larger N2 amplitudes to t trials were associated with higher surgency le the N2 effect was not associated with sur- ency is largely characterized by high intensity fect and approach behavior (Rothbart et al., is often associated with risk-taking behaviors externalizing behavior problems (e.g., Putnam 2005; Rubin et al., 1995; Stifter et al., 2008). ot necessarily thought of as the opposite end nuum of behavioral inhibition or fear, low sur- orted by parents may be indicative of avoidant nsistent with behavioral inhibition and shy- low shyness loads on the surgency factor of the e can think of this surgency measure as reflect- t, low shyness and low avoidance. Although , this finding may suggest that the N2 is sen- peramental shyness in young children much trait anxiety in adults (e.g., Dennis and Chen, now that other ERP components, such as other tal negativities like the ERN, are associated ramental shyness (McDermott et al., 2009) and jcak et al., 2003). However it is important to cDermott and colleagues’ study included ado- o had been previously classified as inhibited direct measure of shyness. Although, recall rson (2010) failed to find a direct association mperamental shyness and N2 in a sample of olds. To date the literature across childhood ood is somewhat mixed in finding an associa- n temperamental shyness and N2. Henderson K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 139 (2010) suggested that from a temperament perspective we should not expect an association between shyness which is considered to be a reactive component of temperament, and N2, which likely reflects a regulatory aspect of tem- perament r 2001). It is ies have fo frontal neg behavior in across seve ing temper be mixed b marker for it appears t a biomarke as effortful vidual diffe 4.3. Limitat The larg small samp precluded o by age and over, given unselected temperame ness. Paren unlikely to todetect th association beendue to scores are b This sugges neural corre than our pr effect and e compelling needed in f ciated with measured o 5. Conclus Findings evidence th in adults, y a developm the N2 only age-related frontal cort As hypothe was associa and reduce was associa gruent N2 a developme of the N2, a ral biomark factors that Author notes The authors wish to thank the Social, Life, and Engineering Sciences Imaging Center (SLEIC), Human Elec- ysiolo quipm coding d a th . is res nal In ded to ublica the Na a com orted b h (PI: anus ences valier, J., rning a , 770–7 , M.T., M amer, A efrontal gnitive nd, O., e averag enceph ction 62 , S., Dun nction a ture Ne , T.S., Ba r cingu hibition , C.S., Br 98. Ante ring of p B.J., Ca rger, S. ht fron ficient/h ild and s, T.A., Ch reat: an s, T.A., M otion r psycholo , McCan e efficie gnitive , T.C., L pedanc urophy in, J.R., V atch on , 152–1 , Russo, layed di , 355–3 .A., He 05. Beh develop 5–262. , J.N., Bl nbos, A ildhood ience 2, an, P.S. e delayeelated to cognitive control (see Rothbart et al., important to note, however, that several stud- und associations with other components of ativities, such as N2 latencies and shy/fearful a clinical sample (e.g., Lewis et al., 2008). So ral studies varying in age and methods assess- amental aspects of behavior there appears to ut promising findings that the N2 may be a individual differences in temperament. In sum, hat modulation of the N2 is a good candidate as r for regulatory aspects of temperament, such processes, and for approach/withdrawal indi- rences. ions e age range of participants resulted in fairly le sizes when age effects were considered. This ur ability to examine associations separately likely reduced power to detect effects. More- that this sample included typically developing, children there was likely a restricted range of ntal variation especially with respect to shy- ts with shy or fearful children may have been volunteer for the study thus limiting our ability ese differences as others have found. Finally, the between N2 and conflict monitoring may have methodoverlap—that is, both theN2and theRT ased on the incongruent–congruent contrast. ts the possibility that the association reflects lates of specific behavioral performance rather oposed theoretical association between the N2 xecutive functioning. Thus, in order to provide evidence for this theoretical link, futurework is uture studies examine whether the N2 is asso- other measures of executive attention that are utside of the ANT context. ions from the current study were consistent with at the N2 reflects conflict monitoring as is does et extends these findings by demonstrating ental effect (i.e., the expected modulation of for children older than 6). This suggests that changes may reflect relatively immature pre- ex development in the preschool-aged children. sized in exploratory predictions, the N2 effect ted with less efficient attention performance d effortful control. Moreover, greater surgency ted with larger N2 effects and larger incon- mplitudes. Taken together, results add to the ntal literature on the morphology and function nd suggest that the N2 holds promise as a neu- er for a range of attentional and temperamental are linked to affective risk and resilience. troph and e with exten study Th Natio awar This p from T.D., supp Healt this m Refer Bache lea 98 Banich Kr pr Co Bertra th tro Se Bishop fu Na Braver rio in Carter 19 to Casey, bu rig de Ch Denni th Denni em ro Fan, J. th Co Ferree im Ne Folste m 45 Fox, E. de 16 Fox, N 20 a 23 Giedd de ch sc Goldm thgyFacility at PennState foruseof the laboratory ent and the Emotion Development Lab for help videos and scoring EEG data. We also wish to ank you to the children who participated in the earch was supported by two grants from the stitutes of Health (NIH) grant 4R01 MH075750, K.B. andgrant5K01MH075764, awarded toT.D. tion was also made possible by Grant RR03037 tional Center for Research Resources (NCRR) to ponent of the NIH. The third author, R.B. was y a grant from the National Institute of Mental Cole T32 MH070327) during the preparation of cript. Mishkin, M., 1984. An early and late developing system for nd retention in infant monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience 78. ilham, M.P., Webb, A., Barad, V., Cohen, N.J., Wszalek, T., .F., 2001. The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Brain Research 12, 467–473. Perrin, F., Pernier, J., 1985. A theoretical justification of e reference in topographic evoked potential studies. Elec- alography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials , 462–464. can, J., Brett, M., Lawrence, A.D., 2004. Prefrontal cortical nd anxiety: controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. uroscience 7, 184–188. rch, D.M., Gray, J.R., Molfese, D.L., Snyder, A., 2001. Ante- late cortex and response to conflict: effects of frequency, and errors. Cerebral Cortex 11, 825–836. aver, T.D., Barch, D.M., Botvinick, M.M., Noll, D., Cohen, J.D., rior cingulate cortex, error detection and the online moni- erformance. Science 280, 747–749. stellanos, F.X., Giedd, J.N., Jay, N., Marsh, W.L., Ham- D., Schubert, A.B., Rapoport, J.L., 1997. Implication of tostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention- yperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 374–383. en, C., 2009. Trait anxiety andconflictmonitoring following ERP study. Psychophysiology 46, 122–131. alone, M.M., Chen, C., 2009. Emotional face processing and egulation in children: an ERP study. Developmental Neu- gy 34, 85–102. dliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., Posner, M.I., 2002. Testing ncy and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Neuroscience 14, 340–347. uu, P., Russell, G.S., Tucker, D.M., 2001. Scalp electrode e, infection risk, and EEG data quality. Journal of Clinical siology 112, 536–544. an Petten, C., 2008. Influence of cognitive control and mis- the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 70. R., Dutton, K., 2002. Attentional bias for threat: evidence for sengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion 79. nderson, H.A., Marshall, P.J., Nichols, K.E., Ghera, M.M., avioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within mental framework. Annual Review of Psychology 56, umenthal, J., Jeffries, N.O., Castellanos, F.X., Hong, L., Zij- ., Paus, T., Rapoport, J.L., 1999. Brain development during and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuro- 861–863. , Rosvold, H.E., Vest, B., Galkin, T.W., 1971. Analysis of d-alternation deficit produced by dorsolateral prefrontal 140 K.A. Buss et al. / Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1 (2011) 131–140 lesions in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative and Physiolog- ical Psychology 77, 212–220. Hajcak, G., McDonald, N., Simons, R.F., 2003. Anxiety and error-related brain activity. Biological Psychology 64, 77–90. Henderson,H.A., 2010. Electrophysiological correlates of cognitive control and the regulation of shyness in children. Developmental Neuropsy- chology 35, 177–193. Hogan, A.M., Vargha-Khadem, F., Kirkham, F.J., Baldeweg, T., 2005. Mat- uration of study. Dev Johnstone, S.J behaviora Journal of Jones, A.D., Ch putational tasks: effe and Behav Junghöfer, M., reference recording. Jonkman, L.M. and inhibi ERP study Kagan, J., Snid Current D Kopp, B., Rist, behaviora 33, 282–2 Ladouceur, C.D toring in a and Oppor Ladouceur, C.D ates relati monitorin 194–211. Lamm, C., Le physiologi conditions Lamm, C., Zela control in of age and Lewis, M.D., St issues and 75, 371–3 Lewis, M.D., G sive childr New York Lewis, M.D., L Neurophy adolescen Lewis, M.D., G 2008. Cha with clinic opment & Lonigan, C.J., and attent Psycholog Lonigan, C.J., V anxiety, an ical Child Luu, P., Tucke tex: limbi (Ed.), Con Benjamins 123–161. Mathews, A., M orders. An McDermott, J. D., Fox, N enhanced anxiety. B Nelson, C.A., allocation response 171–179. Nieuwenhuis, 2003. Elec in a go/no-go task: effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 3, 17–26. Nieuwenhuis, S., Yueng, N., Cohen, J.D., 2004. Stimulus modality, percep- tual overlap, and the go/no-go N2. Psychophysiology 41 (1), 157–160. O’Donnell, S., Noseworthy, M.D., Levine, B., Dennis, M., 2005. Cortical thickness of the frontopolar area in typically developing children and adolescents. Neuroimage 24, 948–954. Olvet, D.M., Hajcak, G., 2008. The error-related negativity (ERN) and psy- opathol , 1343– Edgar, K d Adole Edgar, K en’s per d electr , F., Pern scalp po phy and r,M.I., Pe nual Re r, M.I., R odel for ycholog , S.P., R rms of th sessmen , S.P., R rms of th sessmen , S.P., ers: pre mponen ent 76 (1 art, M.K ddevelo velopm ries on P art, M.K mperam nnaire. , K.H., Co n regula ychopat , M.R., F ., Posne od. Neu n, J., Lew 07. Neu pes of e , 455–4 , C.A., P ddlers: velopm R.M., Lew cial-emo ect and , 595–6 , Hajcak d its mo uropsyc r, D.M., 1 nsor net 5–163. r, D.M., tempor 134–15 een, V., C r: fMRI een, V., ain. Cur i:10.111 ,M.W., D n in ch normal umier, P d emoti RI studyaction monitoring from adolescence to adulthood: an ERP elopmental Science 8, 525–534. ., Pleffer, C.B., Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Smith, J.L., 2005. A l and event-related potential study of children and adults. Psychophysiology 19, 11–23. o, R.Y., Nystrom, L.E., Cohen, J.D., Braver, T.S., 2002. A com- model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response cts of frequency, sequence and conflict. Cognitive, Affective ioral Neuroscience 2, 300–317. Elbert, T., Tucker, D.M., Braun, C., 1999. The polar average effect: A bias in estimating the head surface integral in EEG Clinical Neurophysiology 110, 1149–1155. , 2006. The development of preparation, conflictmonitoring tion from early childhood to young adulthood; a Go/Nogo . Brain Research 1097, 181–193. man, N., Arcus, D.M., 1992. Initial reactions to unfamiliarity. irections in Psychological Science 1, 171–174. R., Mattler, U., 1996. N200 in the flanker task as a neuro- l tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology 94. ., Dahl, R.E., Carter, C.S., 2004. ERP correlates of actionmoni- dolescence. Adolescent Brain Development: Vulnerabilities tunities 1021, 329–336. ., Conway, A., Dahl, R.E., 2010. Attentional control moder- ons between negative affect and neural correlates of action g in adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology 35 (2), wis, M.D., 2010. Developmental change in the neuro- cal correlates of self-regulation in high- and low-emotion . Developmental Neuropsychology 35 (2), 156–176. zo, P.D., Lewis, M.D., 2006. Neural correlates of cognitive childhoodandadolescence: disentangling the contributions executive function. Neuropsychologia 44, 2139–2148. ieben, J., 2004. Emotion regulation in the brain: conceptual directions for developmental research. Child Development 76. ranic, I., Lamm, C., 2006a. Behavioral differences in aggres- en linked with neural mechanisms of emotion regulation. Academy of Sciences 1094, 164–177. amm, C., Segalowitz, S.J., Stieben, J., Zelazo, P.D., 2006b. siological correlates of emotion regulation in children and ts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 430–443. ranic, I., Lamm, C., Zelazo, P.D., Stieben, J., Todd, R.M., et al., nges in the neural bases of emotion regulation associated al improvement in childrenwithbehavior problems.Devel- Psychopathology 20 (3), 913–939. Vasey, M.W., 2009. Negative affectivity, effortful control, ion to threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Child y 37 (3), 387–399. asey, M.W., Phillips, B.M., Hazen, R.A., 2004. Temperament, d the processing of threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clin- and, Adolescent Psychology 33 (1), 8–20. r, D.M., 2004. Self-regulation by the medial frontal cor- c representation of motive set-points. In: Beauregard, M. sciousness, Emotional Self-Regulation and the Brain. John Publishing Company, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, pp. acLeod, C., 2005. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional dis- nual Review of Clinical Psychology 1, 167–195. , Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Pine, ., 2009. A history of childhood behavioral inhibition and response monitoring in adolescence are linked to clinical iological Psychiatry 65, 445–448. Nugent, K.M., 1990. Recognition memory and resource as revealed by children’s event-related potential to happy and angry faces. Developmental Psychology 26, S., Yeung, N., Van Den Wildenberg, W., Ridderinkhof, K.R., trophysiological correlates of anterior cingulated function ch 28 Perez- an Perez- dr an Perrin of ra Posne An Posne m Ps Putnam fo As Putnam fo As Putnam dl co m Rothb an De Se Rothb te tio Rubin tio Ps Rueda L.P ho Stiebe 20 ty 19 Stifter to De Todd, so aff 46 Torpey an Ne Tucke se 14 Tucke tio 1, Van V ito Van V br do Vasey tio Ab Vuille an fMogy: toward an endophenotype. Clinical Psychology Review 1354. ., Fox,N.A., 2005. Temperamentandanxietydisorders. Child scent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 14, 681–706. ., Fox, N.A., 2007. Temperamental contributions to chil- formance in anemotion-worldprocessing task: abehavioral ophysiological study. Brain and Cognition 65, 22–35. ier, J., Bertnard, O., Giard,M.H., Echallier, J.F., 1987.Mapping tentials by surface spline interpolation. Electroencephalog- Clinical Neurophysiology 66, 75–81. tersen, S.E., 1990. The attention systemof the human brain. view of Neuroscience 13, 25–42. othbart, M.K., 2007. Research on attention networks as a the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of y 58, 1–23. othbart, M.K., 2006a. Development of short and very short e children’s behavior questionnaire. Journal of Personality t 87, 103–113. othbart, M.K., 2006b. Development of short and very short e Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality t 87, 102–112. Stifter, C.A., 2005. Behavioral approach-inhibition in tod- diction from infancy, positive and negative affective ts, and relations with behavior problems. Child Develop- ), 212–226. ., Posner, M.I., Hershey, K.L., 1995. Temperament, attention, pmental psychopathology. In:Cicchetti,D., Cohen,D. (Eds.), ental Psychopathology vol. 1: Theory and Methods, Wiley ersonality Processes. Wiley, Oxford, pp. 315–340. ., Ahadi, S.A., Hershey, K.L., Fisher, P., 2001. Investigations of ent at three to seven years: the Children’s Behavior Ques- Child Development 72, 1392–1408. plan, R.J., Fox, N.A., Calkins, S.D., 1995. Emotionality, emo- tion, andpreschoolers’ social adaptation.Development and hology 7 (1), 49–62. an, J., McCandliss, B.D., Halparin, J.D., Gruber, D.B., Lercari, r, M.I., 2004. Development of attentional networks in child- ropsychologia 42, 1029–1040. is, M.D., Granic, I., Zelazo, P.D., Segalowitz, S., Pepler, D., rophysiological mechanisms of emotion regulation for sub- xternalizing children. Development and Psychopathology 80. utnam, S., Jahromi, L., 2008. Exuberant and inhibited stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. ent and Psychopathology 20 (2), 401–421. is, M.D., Meusel, L.A., Zelazo, P.D., 2007. The time course of tional processing in early childhood: ERP response to facial personal familiarity in a go-no go task. Neuropsychologia 13. , Klein, 2009. An examination of error-related brain activity dulation by error value in young children. Developmental hology 36, 749–761. 993. Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: the geodesic . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 87, Liotti, M., Potts, G.F., Russell, G.S., Posner, M.I., 1993. Spa- al analysis of brain electrical fields. Human Brain Mapping 2. arter, C.S., 2002a. The anterior cingulate as a conflict mon- and ERP studies. Physiology and Behavior 77, 477–482. Carter, C.S., 2002b. Conflict and cognitive control in the rent Directions in Psychological Sciences 15, 237–240, 1/j.1467-8721.2006.00443.x. aleiden, E.L.,Williams, L.L., Brown, L.M., 1995. Biased atten- ildhood anxiety disorders: a preliminary study. Journal of Child Psychology 23, 267–279. ., Armony, J., Driver, J., Dolan, R., 2001. Effects of attention on on face processing in the human brain: an event-related . Neuron 30, 829–841.